“…For social scientists studying public opinion and such topics as political culture, the culture wars, ideology, value structure, mass belief systems, partisanship, political sophistication, political participation, policy preferences, risk perceptions, and many other subjects, CT provides rich theoretical resources. Its operationalization in survey research provides fresh insights and often greater explanatory and predictive power than alternative theories and concepts, including left–right ideology and partisanship (see, e.g., Ellis and Thompson, 1997; Gastil et al., 2011; Grendstad, 2000, 2001, 2003a, 2003b; Grendstad and Selle, 1997, 1999; Herron and Jenkins‐Smith, 2006; Jackson, 2014; Johnson and Swedlow, 2019b; Johnson, Swedlow, and Mayorga, 2019; Jones, 2011; Kahan et al., 2010a, 2010b; Kiss, Lachapelle, and Montpetit, 2020; Lachapelle, Montpetit, and Gauvin, 2014; Maleki and Hendriks, 2015; Nowlin and Rabovsky, 2019; Olli, 2012; Ripberger, Jenkins‐Smith, and Herron, 2011; Ripberger et al., 2012, 2014; Swedlow et al., 2016a; Trousset et al., 2015; Wildavksy and Dake, 1990; Xue et al, 2014). However, with the exception of predictive validity (focusing mostly on effect sizes) (Kahan, 2012; Marris et al., 1998; Rippl, 2002; Sjoeberg, 1998, 2003, 2005a, 2005b), the construct validity of the survey measures used in these studies has not begun to be assessed until recently (Cambardella et al., 2020; Johnson and Swedlow, 2019a, 2019b; Johnson, Swedlow, and Mayorga, 2019; Swedlow et al., 2016b; Xue et al., 2014, 2016).…”