2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.tate.2011.10.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acquisition of the concept of teaching and its relationship with theory of mind in French 3- to 6-year olds

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Children age 3 engage in emergent teaching and do so despite their low performance on ToM tasks, as tested by classic false belief tasks and false belief tasks for teaching (Bensalah, Olivier, & Stefaniak, 2012;Davis-Unger & Carlson, 2008a, 2008bStrauss et al, 2002). As teachers, 3 1 / 2 -year-olds predominantly demonstrate how to play a game with little explanation accompanying those demonstrations.…”
Section: Developmental Reliability In Human Ontogenesismentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children age 3 engage in emergent teaching and do so despite their low performance on ToM tasks, as tested by classic false belief tasks and false belief tasks for teaching (Bensalah, Olivier, & Stefaniak, 2012;Davis-Unger & Carlson, 2008a, 2008bStrauss et al, 2002). As teachers, 3 1 / 2 -year-olds predominantly demonstrate how to play a game with little explanation accompanying those demonstrations.…”
Section: Developmental Reliability In Human Ontogenesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children age 5 teach by explaining the game's rules, and that is accompanied by demonstrations (Bensalah et al, 2012;Davis-Unger & Carlson, 2008a, 2008bStrauss et al, 2002). Children at this age also demonstrate the way the game is played, but the dominant teaching strategy is explanation.…”
Section: Developmental Reliability In Human Ontogenesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…During preschool years, children begin to produce causal explanations both in physical and psychological domains and even “teach” their peers by demonstrating causal interventions; the quality of their explanations and interventions increases with age (Ashley & Tomasello, ; Bass et al., ; Bensalah, Olivier, & Stefaniak, ; Rhodes, Bonawitz, Shafto, Chen, & Caglar, ; Strauss, Ziv, & Stein, ; Walker, Lombrozo, Legare, & Gopnik, ; Wellman & Lagattuta, ; Wood, Wood, Ainsworth, & O'Malley, ; see Strauss et al., , for a review). By around 5 years of age, children generate different evidence to teach than to deceive (Rhodes et al., ), provide instructions that address the particular mistakes of the learner (Ronfard & Corriveau, ), prioritize transmitting information that is conventional and causally opaque (e.g., Clegg & Legare, ; Ronfard, Was, & Harris, ), and teach others what they were taught especially when they themselves had difficulty solving the problem (Ronfard et al., ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's performance on the teaching tasks involving teacher's false beliefs correlated with their performance on the false belief task, r = 0.31, controlling for age. This result was since replicated in French children, with a correlation between teaching stories involving false belief and ToM at 0.23 (Bensalah et al, 2012 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 82%