2017
DOI: 10.1044/2017_jslhr-s-16-0125
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Alveolar and Postalveolar Voiceless Fricative and Affricate Productions of Spanish–English Bilingual Children With Cochlear Implants

Abstract: Patterns of fricative and affricate production display effects of bilingualism and diminished signal, yielding unique patterns for bilingual and monolingual CI users.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As for specific speech patterns produced by young bilingual CI users, studies indicate that bilingual children who use CIs can match the productions of their bilingual peers with NH, such as in voice onset time and prevoicing in each language in word-initial position (Bunta, Goodin-Mayeda, Procter, & Hernandez, 2016). There are, however, unique patterns in the productions of bilingual and monolingual CI users that are reflected in differential productions of postalveolar fricatives and affricates showing effects of both bilingual versus monolingual status and CI use versus NH (Li, Bunta, & Tomblin, 2017). Li et al investigated frication duration, frication rise time, and centroid frequency as acoustic cues in production in monolingual and bilingual CI users and their peers with NH.…”
Section: Speech Production In Bilingual Children Who Use Cismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As for specific speech patterns produced by young bilingual CI users, studies indicate that bilingual children who use CIs can match the productions of their bilingual peers with NH, such as in voice onset time and prevoicing in each language in word-initial position (Bunta, Goodin-Mayeda, Procter, & Hernandez, 2016). There are, however, unique patterns in the productions of bilingual and monolingual CI users that are reflected in differential productions of postalveolar fricatives and affricates showing effects of both bilingual versus monolingual status and CI use versus NH (Li, Bunta, & Tomblin, 2017). Li et al investigated frication duration, frication rise time, and centroid frequency as acoustic cues in production in monolingual and bilingual CI users and their peers with NH.…”
Section: Speech Production In Bilingual Children Who Use Cismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies with slightly older children (5-6 years of age) show a more nuanced pattern regarding speech production in children with HL, specifically those with CIs. In general, these studies suggest that children with CIs are able to produce voicing distinctions in stop consonants, as well as their peers with NH (Bunta, Goodin-Mayeda, Procter, & Hernandez, 2016), but fail to demonstrate appropriate spectral structure, specifically in voiceless sibilants (Li, Bunta, & Tomblin, 2017). These findings suggest that children with CIs may have access to veridical temporal structure in the acoustic speech signal, but access only to degraded spectral structure.…”
Section: Perception-production Links In First Speechmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…Tense vowels were included so that formant frequencies could readily be compared in the vocoded and unprocessed conditions; these vowels are preferable to lax vowels, which tend to have less stable formant patterns. Thus, segments were included that readily supported the analyses of temporal and spectral properties, as other investigators have done (Bunta et al, 2016;Li et al, 2017). Nonwords were used as stimuli, because the imitation of real words might be based more on stored representations than on immediate input.…”
Section: Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, the evidence in other target languages indicates that participants with CIs with longer speech support have better speech performance (Geers 2002;Bunta et al 2016). With respect to the factor of language environment, as well as Li, Bunta, and Tomblin (2017), for instance, showed that English-Spanish bilingual children with CIs have a different pattern in their phonological development than monolingual children with CIs. Moreover, bimodal device use has been reported as beneficial for children with CIs' speech development in both quiet and noisy environments (Cuda et al 2019;Yang and Zeng 2017).…”
Section: Individual Differences Factors Affecting Speech Production In Children With Cismentioning
confidence: 95%