2010
DOI: 10.1177/0146167210371316
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Correspondence Bias in Performance Evaluation: Why Grade Inflation Works

Abstract: Performance (such as a course grade) is a joint function of an individual’s ability (such as intelligence) and the situation (such as the instructor’s grading leniency). Prior research has documented a human bias toward dispositional inference, which ascribes performance to individual ability, even when it is better explained through situational influences on performance. It is hypothesized here that this tendency leads admissions decisions to favor students coming from institutions with lenient grading becaus… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
38
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
5
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The increase in co-authoring is likely to persist to the extent that people neglect or underweight co-authorship inflation when they evaluate scholarly productivity. For example, prior studies show that people fail to adjust for grade inflation when they evaluate academic performance, leading them to more favorably evaluate individuals with higher grades, even though these grades reflect semi-arbitrary institutional norms rather than individual ability (Moore, Swift, Sharek, & Gino, 2010;Swift, Moore, Sharek, & Gino, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The increase in co-authoring is likely to persist to the extent that people neglect or underweight co-authorship inflation when they evaluate scholarly productivity. For example, prior studies show that people fail to adjust for grade inflation when they evaluate academic performance, leading them to more favorably evaluate individuals with higher grades, even though these grades reflect semi-arbitrary institutional norms rather than individual ability (Moore, Swift, Sharek, & Gino, 2010;Swift, Moore, Sharek, & Gino, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a more extreme version of the same phenomenon, when an individual read aloud a randomly assigned speech that did not necessarily reflect her opinions on a given topic (e.g., pro-life vs. prochoice), listeners tended to attribute the views expressed to the reader, though they were told the speech had been randomly assigned to the reader (Jones and Harris 1967). Going back to our initial example, even when people are provided with full information about a situation (e.g., average score on the test and standard deviation), they still make biased attributions of the student's ability (Moore et al 2010).…”
Section: Hypothesis Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, suppose that an individual reads about a student's performance on a test and then judges the person's knowledge and ability to decide whether to admit him to a Quizbowl team (Moore et al 2010).…”
Section: Hypothesis Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is exhibited by a majority of American adults and generalizes across demographic characteristics (Bauman and Skitka, 2010). These biased attributions affect a wide variety of social judgments such as performance evaluations (e.g., Moore et al, 2010), blame and guilt judgments (e.g., Kassin & Sukel, 1997), impression formation in social interactions (e.g., Gilbert, 1998;Ross et al, 1977), and judgments of moral character (Bierbrauer, 1979).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%