2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.024
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

69
639
9
21

Year Published

2007
2007
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 783 publications
(738 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
69
639
9
21
Order By: Relevance
“…The contrast between the failure of any of these scales to predict relapse and the predictive significance of several behavioural indices is consistent with other evidence of such dissociations in the context of impulsivity and substance use (e.g. McDonald et al, 2003;Reynolds et al, 2006;Verdejo-Garcia et al, 2008). In particular, Krishnan-Sarin likewise found neurocognitive measures of impulsivity to outperform trait measures in predicting relapse to smoking, as did Goudriaan et al (2008) in relation to gambling.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…The contrast between the failure of any of these scales to predict relapse and the predictive significance of several behavioural indices is consistent with other evidence of such dissociations in the context of impulsivity and substance use (e.g. McDonald et al, 2003;Reynolds et al, 2006;Verdejo-Garcia et al, 2008). In particular, Krishnan-Sarin likewise found neurocognitive measures of impulsivity to outperform trait measures in predicting relapse to smoking, as did Goudriaan et al (2008) in relation to gambling.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Behavioral measures, and not self-reports, were significantly related to treatment outcome, consistent with earlier reports suggesting that behavioral and self-report assessments of impulsivity assessments may represent different levels of analyses, from detailing specific behavioral processes to more general trait-like impulsive tendencies (Lejuez et al, 2003, Reynolds et al, 2006. However, given the small sample size, the differences between selfreport and behavioral measures in predicting treatment outcome should be interpreted with caution.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…"Indifference points," or monetary amounts at which two choice options are treated as subjectively equal, are determined for each block and plotted to form discount functions. Previous research with the EDT has used a hyperbolic-decay function to characterize the pattern of indifference points over increasing delays (Reynolds, 2006;Reynolds & Schiffbauer, 2004). However, an increasingly-common alternative method for analyzing delay-discounting data is to calculate area-under-the-curve (AUC) directly from indifference-point values (Myerson et al, 2001).…”
Section: Experiential Discounting Task (Edt; Reynolds and Schiffbauermentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Although choosing a smaller-sooner sum over a larger-later sum is often described as being an 'impulsive' choice, there is equivocal empirical evidence for (e.g., de Wit, Flory, Acheson, McCloskey, & Manuck, 2007;Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, 1999) and against (e.g., McLeish & Oxoby, 2007;Mitchell, 1999;Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards, & de Wit, 2006) an association between discounting and trait-level impulsivity. This has led to discussion that they have behavioral correlates that can be seen as reflecting impulsive decision making.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%