2016
DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12430
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does the Body Survive Death? Cultural Variation in Beliefs About Life Everlasting

Abstract: Mounting evidence suggests that endorsement of psychological continuity and the afterlife increases with age. This developmental change raises questions about the cognitive biases, social representations, and cultural input that may support afterlife beliefs. To what extent is there similarity versus diversity across cultures in how people reason about what happens after death? The objective of this study was to compare beliefs about the continuation of biological and psychological functions after death in Tan… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
64
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 59 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
3
64
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Astuti and Harris, (), Harris, (, ), and Harris and Giménez, () have argued that afterlife beliefs increase over late childhood and are more likely to be elicited when children are presented with a religious context compared to a non‐religious one. Similar results suggesting that a religious or supernatural context increases afterlife beliefs have been found in several countries including the United States (Lane, Zhu, Evans, & Wellman, ), Spain (Harris & Giménez, ), Madagascar (Astuti, ; Astuti & Harris, ), and Vanuatu (Busch, et al, ; Watson‐Jones, Busch, Harris, & Legare, ). In line with this research, another goal of the current study was to examine the impact of socialization and culture on the prevalence of coexistence beliefs about death in Mexico, a deeply religious country (Lipka, ).…”
Section: Children's Cognitive Understanding Of Deathsupporting
confidence: 73%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Astuti and Harris, (), Harris, (, ), and Harris and Giménez, () have argued that afterlife beliefs increase over late childhood and are more likely to be elicited when children are presented with a religious context compared to a non‐religious one. Similar results suggesting that a religious or supernatural context increases afterlife beliefs have been found in several countries including the United States (Lane, Zhu, Evans, & Wellman, ), Spain (Harris & Giménez, ), Madagascar (Astuti, ; Astuti & Harris, ), and Vanuatu (Busch, et al, ; Watson‐Jones, Busch, Harris, & Legare, ). In line with this research, another goal of the current study was to examine the impact of socialization and culture on the prevalence of coexistence beliefs about death in Mexico, a deeply religious country (Lipka, ).…”
Section: Children's Cognitive Understanding Of Deathsupporting
confidence: 73%
“…These data suggest that for the majority of the children in our sample, a biological understanding coexisted with an alternative spiritual understanding that accepts that dead relatives can return, communicate with the living, and function both biologically and psychologically. Our work adds to a growing body of research suggesting that different ontological categories of belief, often thought to be contradictory, can coexist in the minds of young children Harris & Gim enez, 2005;Legare et al, 2012;Rosengren et al, 2014;Watson-Jones et al, 2017). Harris and Koenig (2006) have suggested that children might be likely to learn from testimony of others about concepts, such as death, that are not readily observable.…”
Section: Coexistence Of Biological and Spiritual Perspectives On Deathmentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Although separating death into these subcomponents has been fruitful, it is limited in its treatment of death as a purely biological concept. This can be problematic because for many individuals and cultures, death is also understood through a religious or spiritual lens (Astuti, ; Gutiérrez et al, ; Watson‐Jones, Busch, Harris, & Legare, ). To examine these nonbiological aspects, some researchers have proposed a fifth subcomponent of death, noncorporeal continuity, which focuses on beliefs in the afterlife (Bering & Bjorklund, ; Bering, Blasi, & Bjorklund, ; Rosengren et al, ).…”
Section: Conceptualizing Deathmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The idea that children actively construct knowledge from available information implies that culture plays a central role in children's emerging understanding of death. Numerous studies have shown cultural differences in how children conceptualize death (Astuti & Harris, 2008;Bering & Bjorklund, 2004;Guti errez et al, 2019;Lane, Zhu, Evans, & Wellman, 2016;Panagiotaki, Nobes, Ashraf, & Aubby, 2015;Watson-Jones et al, 2017). Acknowledging these cultural variations raises the arguably more interesting question of how culture influences children's conceptualizations of death.…”
Section: Biological Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experimental studies conducted in CSR examining how the folk anthropomorphizes supernatural agents by representing them with human-like perceptual abilities have proven to be a mixed bag. Whereas participants are recorded regularly attributing visual and auditory perception to supernatural agents, olfactory, tactile, and gustatory senses are far less frequently attributed to them (Bek & Lock, 2011;Bering, 2002;Bering, Blasi, & Bjorklund, 2005;Nyhof & Johnson, 2017;Shtulman, 2008;Shtulman & Lindeman, 2016;Watson-Jones, Busch, Harris, & Legare, 2017) save when the participants are imagining themselves as surviving their own deaths in an afterlife (Pereira, Faísca, & Sá-Saraiva, 2012). This may, however, be an artifact of how the perceptual questions are asked (Hodge, 2012;Lane, Liqi, Evans, & Wellman, 2016).…”
Section: Anthropomorphism Qua Human-like Mindsmentioning
confidence: 99%