2017
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00316
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Exploring Mechanisms of Selective Directed Forgetting

Abstract: While some studies have shown that providing a cue to selectively forget one subset of previously learned facts may cause specific forgetting of this information, little is known about the mechanisms underlying this memory phenomenon. In three experiments, we aimed to better understand the nature of the selective directed forgetting (SDF) effect. Participants studied a List 1 consisting of 18 sentences regarding two (or three) different characters and a List 2 consisting of sentences regarding an additional ch… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
42
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
5
42
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, these forgotten items show repetition priming on indirect tests ( Basden, 1996 , Basden et al, 1993 , Bjork and Bjork, 1996 , Bjork and Bjork, 2003 , Paller, 1990 ), in contrast with the present findings. One reconciliation of these contrasting results is that list-method directed forgetting impairs recall not by inhibiting individual items, but instead by making people forget the “mental context” of the first list (for supportive neuroimaging and behavioral evidence, see Manning et al, 2016 , Bauml et al, 2010 , Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002 ; however, see evidence for selective directed forgetting that questions the sufficiency of this view; Aguirre et al, 2014 , Aguirre et al, 2017 , Delaney et al, 2009 , Kliegl et al, 2013 ). This context-forgetting process may involve inhibiting contextual representations (e.g., Anderson, 2005 , Anderson and Hanslmayr, 2014 , Bauml et al, 2008 , Bauml et al, 2010 , Hanslmayr et al, 2012 ; or a contrasting view, see Sahakyan et al, 2013 , and Sahakyan & Kelley, 2002 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, these forgotten items show repetition priming on indirect tests ( Basden, 1996 , Basden et al, 1993 , Bjork and Bjork, 1996 , Bjork and Bjork, 2003 , Paller, 1990 ), in contrast with the present findings. One reconciliation of these contrasting results is that list-method directed forgetting impairs recall not by inhibiting individual items, but instead by making people forget the “mental context” of the first list (for supportive neuroimaging and behavioral evidence, see Manning et al, 2016 , Bauml et al, 2010 , Sahakyan and Kelley, 2002 ; however, see evidence for selective directed forgetting that questions the sufficiency of this view; Aguirre et al, 2014 , Aguirre et al, 2017 , Delaney et al, 2009 , Kliegl et al, 2013 ). This context-forgetting process may involve inhibiting contextual representations (e.g., Anderson, 2005 , Anderson and Hanslmayr, 2014 , Bauml et al, 2008 , Bauml et al, 2010 , Hanslmayr et al, 2012 ; or a contrasting view, see Sahakyan et al, 2013 , and Sahakyan & Kelley, 2002 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In their review papers, Nairne and Pandeirada ( 2008 ), Roediger et al ( 2010 ) as well as Anderson and Hanslmayr ( 2014 ) elaborate on motivated forgetting , a mechanism to block fear-inducing memories from becoming conscious, as well as on intentional and directed forgetting. These three concepts have in common that they assume that individuals have executive control processes directed at minimizing the accessibility of memory items and stopping strong habitual responses to cues (Anderson and Green, 2001 ; Aguirre et al, 2017 ; Hu et al, 2017 ). In a current paper by Hu et al ( 2017 ) for example, the authors claim that prior research has shown that suppressing the retrieval of unwanted memory items impairs their retention, as measured with intentional (directed) memory tests (p. 197).…”
Section: State Of the Art Of Forgetting Theories On The Individual Anmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intentional forgetting serves a personal implicit or explicit motive (Bjork et al, 1998 ) or an individual or group-related goal (Harris et al, 2010 ). Many everyday situation require updating knowledge by exerting control over the memory (Aguirre et al, 2017 ). If a memory item is not helpful in a current situation, it should be intentionally forgotten, as it competes with the correct memory item that needs to be applied for goal directed behavior (Aguirre et al, 2017 ).…”
Section: State Of the Art Of Forgetting Theories On The Individual Anmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When changing processes and tasks, the old memory items are not helpful anymore since they compete with the correct (new) memory item. The old items therefore need to be forgotten [41]. The intention to forget by minimizing the accessibility of a memory item or its habitual retrieval leads to an active repression of the cue-target association.…”
Section: Theories Of Learning and Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%