2011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-036x.2011.00636.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Financial Professionals' Overconfidence: Is It Experience, Function, or Attitude?

Abstract: This paper examines financial professionals' overconfidence in their forecasting performance. We compare individuals' self-rating of performance with the true performance, both measured relative to the same peer group. The forecasters in our sample show overconfidence on average, although to a moderate degree, including many cases of underconfidence. In analysing this, we find that working experience is accompanied by less overconfidence. Function is also related to less overconfidence, such as being a fund ma… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
27
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
2
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Since differences in self-perceptions have been attributed to, inter alia, gender stereotypes (Niederle and Vesterlund, 2007) and metacognitive skills (Kruger and Dunning, 1999), we take particular care in allowing for potential gender disparities and gaps in formal education. In doing so, we complement the earlier literature on behavioral biases in financial decisions that has mainly considered the choices of male experts, for instance executive managers (Gervais, Heaton, and Odean, 2011;Adam, Fernando, and Golubeva, 2015;Huang, Tan, and Faff, 2015), portfolio managers (Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014) or traders (Hirshleifer and Luo, 2001;Palomino and Sadrieh, 2011;Sonsino and Regeff, 2013). Less is known about the impact of self-confidence on male and female amateurs' financial decisions and wealth accumulation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…Since differences in self-perceptions have been attributed to, inter alia, gender stereotypes (Niederle and Vesterlund, 2007) and metacognitive skills (Kruger and Dunning, 1999), we take particular care in allowing for potential gender disparities and gaps in formal education. In doing so, we complement the earlier literature on behavioral biases in financial decisions that has mainly considered the choices of male experts, for instance executive managers (Gervais, Heaton, and Odean, 2011;Adam, Fernando, and Golubeva, 2015;Huang, Tan, and Faff, 2015), portfolio managers (Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014) or traders (Hirshleifer and Luo, 2001;Palomino and Sadrieh, 2011;Sonsino and Regeff, 2013). Less is known about the impact of self-confidence on male and female amateurs' financial decisions and wealth accumulation.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…We contribute to a growing and highly relevant research area of experimental studies examining behavioral aspects in financial professionals' decision-making (Alevy et al, 2007;Kaustia et al, 2008;Cipriani and Guarino, 2009;Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014;Cohn et al, 2014;Kirchler et al, 2018). Utilizing an experimental design that allows for a systematic separation of higher moments, potentially driving risk perception and investment propensity, our study facilitates a more comprehensive understanding of how financial professionals assess risks in a financial context in comparison to lay people across nine major economies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, compared to lay people, financial professionals could be expected to perceive risk more analytically along the normative definitions commonly applied in economics and finance models. In addition, an increasing body of experimental evidence suggests that financial professionals are quite "special" in the sense that their behavior systematically differs from other types of experimental subjects, e.g., with regard to anchoring (Kaustia et al, 2008), herding (Cipriani and Guarino, 2009), overconfidence (Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014),…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We contribute to a growing and highly relevant research area of experimental studies examining behavioral aspects in financial professionals' decision-making (Alevy et al, 2007;Kaustia et al, 2008;Cipriani and Guarino, 2009;Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014;Cohn et al, 2014;Kirchler et al, 2018). Utilizing an experimental design that allows for a systematic separation of higher moments, potentially driving risk perception and investment propensity, our study facilitates a more comprehensive understanding of how financial professionals assess risks in a financial context in comparison to lay people across nine major economies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, compared to lay people, financial professionals could be expected to perceive risk more analytically along the normative definitions commonly applied in economics and finance models. In addition, an increasing body of experimental evidence suggests that financial professionals are quite "special" in the sense that their behavior systematically differs from other types of experimental subjects, e.g., with regard to anchoring (Kaustia et al, 2008), herding (Cipriani and Guarino, 2009), overconfidence (Puetz and Ruenzi, 2011;Gloede and Menkhoff, 2014), cheating (Cohn et al, 2014), risk-taking under social comparison and competition (Kirchler et al, 2018), and with respect to public signal updating in information cascades (Alevy et al, 2007). It would thus not be surprising if financial professionals differ in their perception of financial risk.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%