1995
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.746
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Helping children to remember: The influence of object cues on children's accounts of a real event.

Abstract: The authors examined the effects of reinstating objects from an event on 6-and 9-year-old children's reports of the event in which they had either participated or observed. Half of the 95 children were interviewed twice, 10 days and 10 weeks after the event (Group 1), and the remaining children were interviewed a single time, 10 weeks after (Group 2). Following free recall, prompted recall and direct questions were accompanied by objects from the event and distractors for half the children. The effect of the d… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

7
107
4

Year Published

1997
1997
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 118 publications
(118 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(122 reference statements)
7
107
4
Order By: Relevance
“…In light of earlier findings, it was expected that artifact and photo cues would generate more detailed memories (i.e., more ESKs of this event; see Gee & Pipe, 1995;Hudson & Fivush, 1991) than a no-cue (or text) situation, and that odor cues would generate more detailed memories than other cue modalities (see Aggleton & Waskett, 1999). However, the results show that no particular cue type elicited superior recall.…”
Section: Memory-cuing Experimentscontrasting
confidence: 55%
“…In light of earlier findings, it was expected that artifact and photo cues would generate more detailed memories (i.e., more ESKs of this event; see Gee & Pipe, 1995;Hudson & Fivush, 1991) than a no-cue (or text) situation, and that odor cues would generate more detailed memories than other cue modalities (see Aggleton & Waskett, 1999). However, the results show that no particular cue type elicited superior recall.…”
Section: Memory-cuing Experimentscontrasting
confidence: 55%
“…Many of these have been examined in laboratory contexts and involve providing children with either items (Gee & Pipe, 1995;Smith, Ratner, & Hobart, 1987) or representations of items (Goodman & Aman, 1990;Price & Goodman, 1990;Priestley & Pipe, 1997;Salmon, Bidrose, & Pipe, 1995) associated with the to-be-remembered event, revisiting the context in which the event occurred (Pipe & Wilson, 1994;Priestley, Roberts, & Pipe, 1999;Wilkinson, 1988), or drawing during interviews (Butler, Gross, & Hayne, 1995;Gross & Hayne, 1998). Although these techniques enhance the amount of information retrieved, at least under some conditions (see Salmon, 2001, for a review), interviewers seldom know which props might be relevant, and need to avoid introducing props at times which would make them suggestive.…”
Section: Probing Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, reinstating interviews had little effect on 6-year-old children's recall of a magic lesson (Gee & Pipe, 1995), even a year later (Pipe, Gee, Wilson, & Egerton, 1999), or on young children's recall of a pediatric checkup (Ornstein et al, 2006), or on 3-to 5-year-olds' memory of playing in a lab (Quas et al, 2007). However, it is important to emphasize that in these studies, there was little suggestive questioning or misinformation introduced, and so these issues are not considered here.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%