2016
DOI: 10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

9
115
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 148 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
9
115
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, some studies have not been able to capture the effect of LSF on the task-switching ability when comparing bilingual groups differing in how often individuals switch languages. For example, both Paap et al (2017) and Yim and Bialystok (2012) failed to find an association between their LSF measures and nonlinguistic task-switching costs. In contrast, yet similarly to Barbu et al (2018), we found that those participants who switched languages more often were faster (i.e., more efficient) when switching in a non-verbal switching task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, some studies have not been able to capture the effect of LSF on the task-switching ability when comparing bilingual groups differing in how often individuals switch languages. For example, both Paap et al (2017) and Yim and Bialystok (2012) failed to find an association between their LSF measures and nonlinguistic task-switching costs. In contrast, yet similarly to Barbu et al (2018), we found that those participants who switched languages more often were faster (i.e., more efficient) when switching in a non-verbal switching task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Nevertheless, recent reports regarding the relation between bilingualism and EC are heterogeneous and mixed (Sanchez-Azanza et al, 2017), contributing to the debate on the key factors involving bilingualism-related effects on EC. For instance, there are also several studies showing findings that are in conflict or nuance the literature presented in the two previous paragraphs regarding the AoA (Vega-Mendoza et al, 2015), PL2 (Pelham & Abrams, 2014) and LSF (Paap et al, 2017) factors. One of the possible reasons for this mixture of results might be the divergent sample characteristics, in particular regarding the bilingual groups (Luk & Bialystok, 2013;.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…The opponents of this hypothesis have failed to replicate this advantage in young adults [82,83], children [84], and the elderly [85], and have hence suggested that such advantage may be a product of factors other than bilingualism proper (for example, small sample sizes and inconsistencies in the methods [86,87], for a response see [88]; or a result of a publication bias [89], for a response see [90]; for a thorough review of the evidence for and against the bilingual advantage hypothesis please refer to [91]).…”
Section: Experience Dependent Bilingual Advantagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet despite the evidence reported in favor of an advantage, recent investigations have cast doubt over whether bilinguals really do outperform monolinguals on these tasks, and whether it is necessarily bilingualism that is the reason when they do (e.g., Antón et al., ; Antón, García, Carreiras, & Duñabeitia, ; Duñabeitia et al., ; Hilchey, Saint‐Aubin, & Klein, ; Morton & Harper, ; Paap, ; Paap & Greenberg, ; Paap, Johnson, & Sawi, , , ; Paap et al., ). For example, large‐scale studies of bilingual and monolingual children failed to find any evidence of an advantage on the ANT (Antón et al., ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%