2011
DOI: 10.5334/jpl.101
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phrasal prosody constrains word segmentation in French 16-month-olds

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A long series of studies shows that infants develop an impressive expertise with prosody from their first days of life. Newborns are able to exploit rhythmic information to discriminate between languages (Mehler et al, 1988;Nazzi, Bertoncini, & Mehler, 1998); from 4.5 months onwards, infants are sensitive to the coherence of prosodic constituents (Gerken, Jusczyk, & Mandel, 1994;Hirsh-Pasek et al, 1987;Jusczyk, Hohne, & Mandel, 1995;Männel & Friederici, 2009;Soderstrom, Seidl, Nelson, & Jusczyk, 2003), they show better recognition and memory for segments that correspond to whole prosodic units than for those which span prosodic boundaries (Mandel, Jusczyk, & Nelson, 1994;Nazzi, Iakimova, Bertoncini, Frédonie, & Alcantara, 2006) and they can rely on prosodic cues to segment the speech stream into words and constrain their lexical access (Gout, Christophe, & Morgan, 2004;Johnson, 2008;Millotte et al, 2010;Shukla, White, & Aslin, 2011). All of these findings, together with the reliable relationship between prosodic and syntactic structures, suggest that toddlers might be able to use phrasal prosody, not only to facilitate memory and lexical access, but also to constrain their syntactic analysis (see Christophe et al, 2008;Hawthorne & Gerken, 2014;Massicotte-Laforge & Shi, 2015;Morgan & Demuth, 1996;Morgan, 1986;Shi, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A long series of studies shows that infants develop an impressive expertise with prosody from their first days of life. Newborns are able to exploit rhythmic information to discriminate between languages (Mehler et al, 1988;Nazzi, Bertoncini, & Mehler, 1998); from 4.5 months onwards, infants are sensitive to the coherence of prosodic constituents (Gerken, Jusczyk, & Mandel, 1994;Hirsh-Pasek et al, 1987;Jusczyk, Hohne, & Mandel, 1995;Männel & Friederici, 2009;Soderstrom, Seidl, Nelson, & Jusczyk, 2003), they show better recognition and memory for segments that correspond to whole prosodic units than for those which span prosodic boundaries (Mandel, Jusczyk, & Nelson, 1994;Nazzi, Iakimova, Bertoncini, Frédonie, & Alcantara, 2006) and they can rely on prosodic cues to segment the speech stream into words and constrain their lexical access (Gout, Christophe, & Morgan, 2004;Johnson, 2008;Millotte et al, 2010;Shukla, White, & Aslin, 2011). All of these findings, together with the reliable relationship between prosodic and syntactic structures, suggest that toddlers might be able to use phrasal prosody, not only to facilitate memory and lexical access, but also to constrain their syntactic analysis (see Christophe et al, 2008;Hawthorne & Gerken, 2014;Massicotte-Laforge & Shi, 2015;Morgan & Demuth, 1996;Morgan, 1986;Shi, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, infants exploit phrasal prosody to identify their mother tongue from birth onwards (e.g. Mehler, Jusczyk, Lamsertz, Halsted, Bertoncini et al ., ; Nazzi, Bertoncini & Mehler, ), they are sensitive to the coherence of prosodic constituents (at 4 months, for intonational phrases; Hirsh‐Pasek, Kemler Nelson, Jusczyk, Cassidy, Druss et al ., ; from 6 months on, for smaller prosodic units; Gerken, Jusczyk & Mandel, ; Soderstrom, Seidl, Nelson & Jusczyk, ), they show better memory for whole prosodic units than for chunks that span prosodic boundaries (Mandel, Jusczyk & Nelson, ; Nazzi, Iakimova, Bertoncini & Alcantara, ) and they use prosodic boundaries to constrain lexical access by 10 months of age (Gout, Christophe & Morgan, ; Johnson, ; Millotte, Margules, Dutat, Bernal & Christophe, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, EP sound structure offers stronger cues for word segmentation than those available in other Romance languages, although weaker then in English (Vigário, 2003). These and other languagespecific patterns may impact on the acquisition of words and related language development (Demuth, 2006;Millotte et al, 2010;Vigário et al, 2006).…”
Section: ) and Galicianmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, some languages have shown a different pace in development, e.g., Danish and French children exhibit a slower pace, and American English children show a faster pace in development (Bleses et al, 2008;Millotte et al, 2010;Wehberg et al, 2007). It has been hypothesized that at least some of these differences might be related to linguistic factors, namely to aspects of sound structure like strong reduction in Danish and absence of word boundary cues in French (but see Hamilton et al, 2000 for the potential role of social/cultural variables).…”
Section: ) and Galicianmentioning
confidence: 99%