2019
DOI: 10.1007/s11049-019-09449-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Subextraction in Japanese and subject-object symmetry

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, there is a problem that the length of the scrambled phrases differed between the D-link ("sono NP-o") and non-D-link ("NP-o") conditions. According to corpus studies and acceptability judgement studies, long objects tent to be placed before short subjects, known as the long-before-short preference (Dryer 1980;Hawkins 1994;Yamashita & Chang 2001;Yamashita 2002;Omaki et al 2019). This leaves the possibility that the sentences with demonstratives received the significantly higher mean acceptability judgments because they are more compatible with the preference.…”
Section: Remaining Problemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, there is a problem that the length of the scrambled phrases differed between the D-link ("sono NP-o") and non-D-link ("NP-o") conditions. According to corpus studies and acceptability judgement studies, long objects tent to be placed before short subjects, known as the long-before-short preference (Dryer 1980;Hawkins 1994;Yamashita & Chang 2001;Yamashita 2002;Omaki et al 2019). This leaves the possibility that the sentences with demonstratives received the significantly higher mean acceptability judgments because they are more compatible with the preference.…”
Section: Remaining Problemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition to the simple presence of a wh-dependency, the length of the dependency also has an effect on acceptability, with greater distance (in terms of syntactic structure) resulting in a larger degradation. This was 3 Hofmeister, Culicover, and Winkler (2015) provide another very clear example of how the simple presence of a whdependency results in significant degradation, as do Omaki et al (2020) with regard to the presence of a scrambling dependency in Japanese. See also Namboodiripad (2017).…”
Section: Length Of Dependencymentioning
confidence: 97%
“…This asymmetry in island sensitivity between in-situ wh-DPs and wh-adverbs mentioned above has been established mostly based on informal judgments by syntacticians. However, recent experimental studies on wh-in-situ languages put this generalization into question: Kim and Goodall (2016) found that Korean wh-DPs are sensitive to wh-islands; Omaki et al (2020) found that Japanese in-situ wh-adverbs are equally insensitive to subject islands as in-situ wh-DPs; Lu et al (2020) found that Chinese wh-DPs and wh-adverbs are both sensitive to relative clause islands. 2 Crucially, Lu et al (2020) pointed out that the acceptability contrasts between wh-DPs and wh-adverbs inside island structures (e.g., the contrast between (3/4a) and (3/4b)) was due to a penalty of long distance covert movement of wh-adverbs, which has nothing to do with island sensitivity.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%