2007
DOI: 10.1080/13682820600988777
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Subject–verb agreement and phonological processing in developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment (SLI): a closer look

Abstract: The results show similarities between the performances of children with SLI and dyslexia on tasks tapping subject-verb agreement, phonological awareness, and non-word repetition: they scored more poorly than typically developing children. Qualitative analyses revealed, however, differences in the error patterns on all three tasks. Associations between non-word repetition and sensitivity to subject-verb agreement were found, suggesting that problems with phonological processing impact on morphosyntactic skills.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

13
63
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(79 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
13
63
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Previous research on the acquisition of subject-verb agreement in TD Dutch shows that children generally correctly express verbal agreement from the moment they begin to produce two-word utterances, between the ages of 2 and 3 (Blom 2003). As for Dutch-speaking children with SLI, it is well known that they experience problems producing correctly inflected finite verbs de (de Jong 1999;Rispens & Been 2007). Whereas TD children no longer produce root infinitives (i.e., mama lopen 'mommy walk') by age 3 (Blom 2003), children with SLI continue to do so until the age of 8 (Wexler, Schaeffer & Bol 2004).…”
Section: Personmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research on the acquisition of subject-verb agreement in TD Dutch shows that children generally correctly express verbal agreement from the moment they begin to produce two-word utterances, between the ages of 2 and 3 (Blom 2003). As for Dutch-speaking children with SLI, it is well known that they experience problems producing correctly inflected finite verbs de (de Jong 1999;Rispens & Been 2007). Whereas TD children no longer produce root infinitives (i.e., mama lopen 'mommy walk') by age 3 (Blom 2003), children with SLI continue to do so until the age of 8 (Wexler, Schaeffer & Bol 2004).…”
Section: Personmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our study has targeted number agreement, but it is unlikely that the grammatical problems of deaf readers will be limited to this domain. Looking beyond the deaf population, our study, along with other recent research (Rispens & Been, 2007;Oakhill, Cain & Bryant, 2003), forces us to consider the possibility that grammatical knowledge is an important contributor to reading success and that problems with grammatical knowledge could also contribute to literacy impairment in other populations, for example in dyslexia. Intervention studies should be performed to test whether training that targets morphological relationships at the level of sentence processing will move levels of literacy forward in deaf learners.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…For example, monitoring for agreement errors is disrupted by a cognitive load more than monitoring for problems with word order or local plural agreement (e.g. two boys/one boy; McDonald, 2008), subject verb agreement is specifically disrupted in specific language impairment and dyslexia (Rice & Oetting, 1993;Rispens and Been, 2007;Leonard, Bortolini, Caselli, McGregor & Sabbadini, 1992), and acquisition of tense and number agreement is relatively late compared to other function morphemes (Brown, 1973;Menyuk, 1969;Cazden, 1968).…”
Section: Subject-verb Number Agreementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One area that is typically influenced by the impairment is morphosyntax and the ability to comprehend and produce grammatical morphemes. These grammatical morphemes include those that indicate tense, number marking and subject-verb agreement (Rice, Wexler & Cleave 1995;Clahsen, Bartke & Göllner 1997;de Jong 1999;Wexler, Schaeffer & Bol 2004;Rispens & Been 2007;Spoelman & Bol 2012). For example, Clahsen et al (1997) compare 9 Englishspeaking children with SLI to 6 German-speaking children with SLI aged between 5 and 8 years old on subject-verb agreement and tense.…”
Section: Specific Language Impairmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We know from previous studies that children with specific language impairment (SLI) have difficulties with the comprehension and production of grammatical morphemes. For example, Dutch-speaking children with SLI have been shown to have problems with subject-verb agreement or finiteness both in judgment (Rispens & Been 2007) and in production (de Jong 1999;Wexler, Schaeffer & Bol 2004). Less is known about the difficulties that children with SLI experience with morphology in the nominal domain (but cf.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%