1992
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90003-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The perception of globally coherent motion

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
73
0

Year Published

1999
1999
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 107 publications
(81 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
8
73
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, in the absence of contrast or depth cues, or special features like terminators, the visual system defaults to simple pooling over space to produce estimates of global velocity (Mingolla et al, 1992;Rubin and Hochstein, 1993;Lorenceau and Zago, 1999). Neurons in MT behave similarly, their response to spatially separate features presented within their receptive fields being the average of their responses to each of the features presented alone, whether the features differ in contrast (Britten and Heuer, 1999;Heuer and Britten, 2002) or direction of motion (Britten and Newsome, 1990;Ferrera and Lisberger, 1997;Recanzone et al, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, in the absence of contrast or depth cues, or special features like terminators, the visual system defaults to simple pooling over space to produce estimates of global velocity (Mingolla et al, 1992;Rubin and Hochstein, 1993;Lorenceau and Zago, 1999). Neurons in MT behave similarly, their response to spatially separate features presented within their receptive fields being the average of their responses to each of the features presented alone, whether the features differ in contrast (Britten and Heuer, 1999;Heuer and Britten, 2002) or direction of motion (Britten and Newsome, 1990;Ferrera and Lisberger, 1997;Recanzone et al, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are several theories which speculate on how and where pattern motion is computed from V1 outputs. One idea is that pattern motion is computed in MT (Adelson and Movshon 1982;Albright 1984;Heeger 1987;Movshon et al 1985;Simoncelli and Heeger 1998) where the V1 outputs are combined using the intersection of constraints (IOC) rule (Adelson and Movshon 1982;Simoncelli and Heeger 1998) or vector averaging (Mingolla et al 1992;Rubin and Hochstein 1993). Another idea is that end-stopped cells in V1 could be involved in encoding pattern motion because they respond well to line terminators (or features) moving in their preferred direction and speed, independent of the orientation of the contour (Pack and Born 2001;Pack et al 2003;Born and Bradley 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Due to the aperture problem, outputs from the directional transient cell network (equation 3.3) do not unambiguously signal the direction of object motion (Marr & Ullman, 1981;Wallach, 1935;Wuerger et al, 1996). Cross-directional normalizing competition enhances the least ambiguous regions and suppresses the most ambiguous regions, thus strengthening feature tracking signals which help reduce the effects of the aperture problem (Bayerl & Neumann, 2004;Berzhanskaya et al, 2007;Chey et al, 1997;Lucas & Kanade, 1981;Mingolla, Todd & Norman, 1992). .2), scale 1 is at the original input resolution, scale 2 is reduced by a factor of 2, and scale 3 is reduced by a factor of 4.…”
Section: Level 1: On-center Off-surround Network (γ)mentioning
confidence: 99%