1989
DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2407(08)60414-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Understanding Maps as Symbols: The Development of Map Concepts in Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
96
0
3

Year Published

1999
1999
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 130 publications
(101 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
2
96
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…When asked to place colored stickers on a map in locations that corresponded to colored flags on a three-dimensional model of the same terrain, only 27% of the first graders' stickers and 40% of the second graders' stickers ended up in approximately the correct location (Liben & Downs, 1989). These and other research results lead to the conclusion that skilled map use is not an ability that develops naturally and inevitably in all children, like walking or talking.…”
mentioning
confidence: 77%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…When asked to place colored stickers on a map in locations that corresponded to colored flags on a three-dimensional model of the same terrain, only 27% of the first graders' stickers and 40% of the second graders' stickers ended up in approximately the correct location (Liben & Downs, 1989). These and other research results lead to the conclusion that skilled map use is not an ability that develops naturally and inevitably in all children, like walking or talking.…”
mentioning
confidence: 77%
“…We made this judgment based on the recorded comments of students who did the flag-sticker activity in pairs during the pilot stage of the project and on prior research showing that locations on unique map symbols are easier than those on repeated symbols (e.g., Blades & Spencer, 1994;Liben & Downs, 1989), and locations proximal to landmarks are easier than locations remote from landmarks (Siegel, 1981).…”
Section: The Flag-sticker Field-based Map Skills Testmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Confirmation that children are more likely to find interpretation difficult when there are conflicts between the properties of referent and representation comes from studies of children's understanding and judgments about maps (Liben, 1999;Liben & Downs, 1989). Children are able, for example, to identify correctly a river on a map because "it is blue," but become confused by (nonrealistic) coloring of main roads as red and built-up areas as yellow.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Not only we should study if 'this type of 3D is good for task type x' (for what) but we should study if 'this type of 3D is good for task type x and participant type y' (for whom). Literature abounds with examples that expertise, i.e., education, experience, previous exposure (e.g., Çöltekin et al, 2010), spatial abilities (Liben & Downs, 1989;Huk, 2006), visual abilities (Fukuda et al, 2010), age (Schnürer et al, 2015), and possibly other characteristics, such as lack of sleep (Kong et al, 2011), or if the alphabet is pictorial/iconic, or if one reads and writes from left to right, or if one conventionally uses certain graphical designs, etc.) all have an influence on whether we benefit from working different kinds of graphics (including 3D), or not.…”
Section: Conceptual Organization Of Current Empirical Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%