2012
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1117465109
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Variability in encoding precision accounts for visual short-term memory limitations

Abstract: It is commonly believed that visual short-term memory (VSTM) consists of a fixed number of "slots" in which items can be stored. An alternative theory in which memory resource is a continuous quantity distributed over all items seems to be refuted by the appearance of guessing in human responses. Here, we introduce a model in which resource is not only continuous but also variable across items and trials, causing random fluctuations in encoding precision. We tested this model against previous models using two … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

52
806
6
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 526 publications
(903 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
52
806
6
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants' memory thus seems to be capacity-limited as predicted by a slot-model. In this and the following literature, fixed and limited capacities have thus been used as diagnostic of slotlike memory representations vs. continuous resource-like representations, often based on sophisticated mathematical analyses (e.g., van den Berg et al, 2012;Rouder et al, 2008; W. Zhang & Luck, 2008).…”
Section: Slot-vs Resource-based Models Of Wmmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Participants' memory thus seems to be capacity-limited as predicted by a slot-model. In this and the following literature, fixed and limited capacities have thus been used as diagnostic of slotlike memory representations vs. continuous resource-like representations, often based on sophisticated mathematical analyses (e.g., van den Berg et al, 2012;Rouder et al, 2008; W. Zhang & Luck, 2008).…”
Section: Slot-vs Resource-based Models Of Wmmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Examples include Endress and Potter's (2014a) interference-rich conditions, or the change detection paradigm (Luck & Vogel, 1997) that has probably become the most prominent test case of visual WM in recent years (e.g., Alvarez & Cavanagh, 2004;van den Berg et al, 2012;Piazza et al, 2011;Rouder et al, 2008;Vogel et al, 2006;W. Zhang & Luck, 2008).…”
Section: The Role Of Active Mechanismsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several computational models already exist for visual WM (Bays, 2014;Matthey, Bays, & Dayan, 2015;Swan & Wyble, 2014;van den Berg, Shin, Chou, George, & Ma, 2012). We recently proposed a computational model of attention to items in verbal WM (Oberauer, 2013;Oberauer, Souza, Druey, & Gade, 2013) that explains the effects of switching attention between items in WM and of swapping entire memory sets into and out of WM.…”
Section: Outlook and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After a short delay period, participants report the hue value of one of the study objects on a con-4 tinuous response scale, a hue circle (usually with 180 exemplars) comprising all the hues utilized in the study. Response variability -measured as angular deviation between selected and true hues-differs between trials and by condition, motivating inferences concerning the structure of visual working memory (Anderson & Awh, 2012;Bays, Catalao, & Husain, 2009;Bays, Wu, & Husain, 2011;Emrich & Ferber, 2011;Fougnie & Alvarez, 2011;Fougnie, Asplund, & Marois, 2010;Fougnie, Suchow, & Alvarez, 2012;Gold et al, 2010;van den Berg, Shin, Chou, George, & Ma, 2012;Wilken & Ma, 2004;Zhang & Luck, 2011;2009;.…”
Section: Delayed Estimationmentioning
confidence: 99%