2004
DOI: 10.1038/nrn1476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visual objects in context

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

81
1,302
6
38

Year Published

2008
2008
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,424 publications
(1,427 citation statements)
references
References 135 publications
81
1,302
6
38
Order By: Relevance
“…Graf et al suggested that the first object in the sequence adjusts the orientation of an abstract reference frame in which the second object is initially processed. However, the preceding context may also provide semantic and structural information that could facilitate the recognition of the object (Bar, 2004;Biederman et al, 1982) in addition to orientation priming. Bar (2004) recently proposed a physiologically motivated model of object recognition in which he suggested that the scene context may facilitate the initial processing of the objectfor example, by limiting the search space during object recognition.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Graf et al suggested that the first object in the sequence adjusts the orientation of an abstract reference frame in which the second object is initially processed. However, the preceding context may also provide semantic and structural information that could facilitate the recognition of the object (Bar, 2004;Biederman et al, 1982) in addition to orientation priming. Bar (2004) recently proposed a physiologically motivated model of object recognition in which he suggested that the scene context may facilitate the initial processing of the objectfor example, by limiting the search space during object recognition.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bar (2004) suggested that the "gist" of a scene is processed very quickly with low spatial frequencies and that this serves to restrict the search space for possible objects. Hollingworth and Henderson (1998), on the other hand, found no object-scene congruency advantage when they repeated the Biederman et al (1982) study and controlled for position cues and response bias.…”
Section: Context Influences On Object Classificationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An important source of contextual information is global scene representation (Bar, 2004;Oliva and Torralba, 54 2007). Therefore, feedback may use coarse information carried via LSF.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Effects of emotion and memory on scene recognition 30 consistent with the assumption that this region is rapidly involved in the analysis of diagnostic 653 contextual information, primarily conveyed by magnocellular input (Bar, 2009(Bar, , 2007(Bar, , 2004. 654…”
Section: Emotion-dependent Monotonic Accumulation Of Perceptual Evidementioning
confidence: 61%
“…Our new ERP 703 findings are consistent with a recent theoretical proposal (Barrett and Bar, 2009) suggesting that 704 the affective properties of the visual input are intrinsically embedded in the predictions generated 705 during the rapid extraction of its gist. Given the direct anatomical projections from specific 706 medial frontal brain areas (including the ACC and insula) --active during recognition processes--707 to autonomic and endocrine output centers in the hypothalamus, midbrain, and brainstem, 708 internal affective states of the organism might potentially be one feature of the set of predictions 709 used to facilitate recognition (Bar and Aminoff, 2003;Bar, 2004;Barrett and Bar, 2009). …”
Section: Emotion-dependent Monotonic Accumulation Of Perceptual Evidementioning
confidence: 99%