2008
DOI: 10.1080/02699050802403565
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Working memory performance following paediatric traumatic brain injury

Abstract: Performance- and rater-based working memory measures, while not significantly correlated, are both sensitive to acquired cognitive dysfunction following paediatric traumatic brain injury. Demographic and clinical factors may be used to predict cognitive outcomes, educate caregivers and design clinical interventions.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
49
1

Year Published

2011
2011
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
7
49
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the developers of the BRIEF caution that the results need to be utilized in the context of other instruments such as performance-based measures (Isquith et al, 2005). However, although BRIEF scores have been associated with various behavior-rating scales (e.g., Denckla, 2002;Mahone & Hoffman, 2007;McAuley, Chen, Goos, Schachar, & Crosbie, 2010), low and nonsignificant correlations are usually found with direct EF measures (Conklin, Salorio, & Slomine, 2008;McAuley et al, 2010;Toplak et al, 2013;Vriezen & Pigott, 2002). In preschoolers, relationships between the BRIEF-P and a more general behavioral scale (the Child Behavior Checklist; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) provide evidence of convergent validity (Duku & Vaillancourt, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Finally, the developers of the BRIEF caution that the results need to be utilized in the context of other instruments such as performance-based measures (Isquith et al, 2005). However, although BRIEF scores have been associated with various behavior-rating scales (e.g., Denckla, 2002;Mahone & Hoffman, 2007;McAuley, Chen, Goos, Schachar, & Crosbie, 2010), low and nonsignificant correlations are usually found with direct EF measures (Conklin, Salorio, & Slomine, 2008;McAuley et al, 2010;Toplak et al, 2013;Vriezen & Pigott, 2002). In preschoolers, relationships between the BRIEF-P and a more general behavioral scale (the Child Behavior Checklist; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) provide evidence of convergent validity (Duku & Vaillancourt, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…It consists of behavioral regulation and metacognition indexes that have been identified by factor analysis of individual subscales. The three overall indexes (general executive composite, metacognition index, behavioral regulation index) have been shown to be sensitive to TBI severity and outcome Chevignard et al, 2009;Conklin et al, 2008;Donders et al, 2010;Gioia and Isquith, 2004;Gioia et al, 2002Gioia et al, , 2010Karunanayaka et al, 2007;Maillard-Wermelinger et al, 2009;Mangeot et al, 2002;Merkley et al, 2008;Muscara et al, 2008a, b;Nadebaum et al, 2007;Power et al, 2007;Sesma et al, 2008;Vriezen and Pigott, 2002;Walz et al, 2008;Wozniak et al, 2007). The BRIEF was selected as a supplemental measure to provide an evaluation of everyday executive function and because of its standardization on a large number of typically developing children, thus providing age-based standard scores.…”
Section: Behavior Rating Inventory Of Executive Function (Brief)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[1][2][3][4] Deficits in working memory (WM) are a common consequence of pediatric TBI [5][6][7][8][9][10] and are believed to contribute to difficulties in a range of cognitive and academic domains, including discourse and reading comprehension, mathematics, complex learning, and reasoning. [11][12][13] Neural networks in WM WM is the capacity-limited ability to monitor, process, and maintain task-relevant information online to respond to immediate environmental demands.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%