Handbuch Der Dialoganalyse
DOI: 10.1515/9783110940282.177
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

10. Grundlagen der Dialogorganisation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0
3

Publication Types

Select...
2
2
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
0
2
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The activation in these regions is best explained by the action-related nature of a Request. Since Requests are embedded in an action sequence structure (Fritz and Hundsnurscher, 1994), which is more complex in the case of a Request than in Naming (see “Introduction” and Figure 1), the fronto-central cortex could be the neural basis supporting the knowledge about and implementation of these action-sequences. Sequence processing and computation of hierarchical action structure is likely to be carried out by Broca's area and premotor cortex (in interaction with other areas and functions; see, e.g., Pulvermüller and Fadiga, 2010) in a way similar to the storage of action-semantics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The activation in these regions is best explained by the action-related nature of a Request. Since Requests are embedded in an action sequence structure (Fritz and Hundsnurscher, 1994), which is more complex in the case of a Request than in Naming (see “Introduction” and Figure 1), the fronto-central cortex could be the neural basis supporting the knowledge about and implementation of these action-sequences. Sequence processing and computation of hierarchical action structure is likely to be carried out by Broca's area and premotor cortex (in interaction with other areas and functions; see, e.g., Pulvermüller and Fadiga, 2010) in a way similar to the storage of action-semantics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A speech act (Austin, 1962; Searle, 1969; Van Dijk, 1977; Fritz and Hundsnurscher, 1994; Horn and Ward, 2006; Ehlich, 2010) can be characterized by specifying (1) the linguistic utterance used to perform it; (2) the physical setting in which the communicative actors find themselves; (3) the intentions and assumptions the actors commit themselves to; and (4) the action sequence structures in which the speech act is typically embedded (i.e., actions occurring with the speech act, including those preceding and following it). As there are many different speech acts, (for a systematic classification see Bach and Harnish, 1979; Searle, 1979), we here focus on two prototypical examples, the speech acts of Naming and Requesting (Figure 1).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dabei werden die in den überlieferten Texten manifesten Traditionen des Sprechens und ihre sprachlichen Realisierungen als historisch bestimmte Mittel zur Lösung kommunikativer Aufgaben gedeutet (cf. Fritz 1994.…”
Section: Unterschiedliche Wurzeln Und Gemeinsame Herausforderungenunclassified
“…Nach Coseriu ist das Sprechen «eine universelle allgemein-menschliche Tätigkeit, die jeweils von individuellen Sprechern als Vertretern von Sprachgemeinschaften mit gemeinschaftlichen Traditionen des Sprechenkönnens individuell in bestimmten Situationen realisiert wird» (Coseriu 1988, 70). Diese Parameter des Sprechens werden nun auf drei Ebenen des Sprechens projiziert, denen bekanntlich drei Typen des Wissens entsprechen (Coseriu , 2000Schema nach Coseriu 2000, 89): 27 Zum Vergleich Fritz 1994Fritz , 1995Fritz , 1997, der ein Programm für die historische Dialoganalyse entwirft. 28 Den Vorwurf an die Pragmalinguistik, ein fourre-tout zu sein, diskutieren etwa Mey 2 Auf der universellen Ebene des Sprechens im allgemeinen ist das «elokutionelle Wissen» situiert, das allgemein gültige Regeln des Sprechens erfasst, wie sie sich aus den Universalien der Rede, wie etwa der Semantizität und Alterität des Sprechens, ergeben.…”
Section: Modelle Und Deutungsmusterunclassified
See 1 more Smart Citation