2007
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.54
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

12-month-old infants allocate increased neural resources to stimuli associated with negative adult emotion.

Abstract: Young infants use caregivers' emotional expressions to guide their behavior in novel, ambiguous situations. This skill, known as social referencing, likely involves at least 3 separate abilities: (a) looking at an adult in an unfamiliar situation, (b) associating that adult's emotion with the novel situation, and (c) regulating their own emotions in response to the adult's emotional display. The authors measured each of these elements individually as well as how they related to each other. The results revealed… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

8
63
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 68 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
8
63
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These results are generally consistent with observational learning (Carver & Vaccaro, 2007;Mumme, Fernald, & Herrera, 1996) and classical conditioning studies, showing that reward learning does not produce as strong or as reliable effects as fear learning. This may be because reward stimuli used in experiments are not as arousing as commonly used aversive stimuli, and therefore do not engage the amygdala in learning.…”
Section: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms Of Observational Learningsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…These results are generally consistent with observational learning (Carver & Vaccaro, 2007;Mumme, Fernald, & Herrera, 1996) and classical conditioning studies, showing that reward learning does not produce as strong or as reliable effects as fear learning. This may be because reward stimuli used in experiments are not as arousing as commonly used aversive stimuli, and therefore do not engage the amygdala in learning.…”
Section: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms Of Observational Learningsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Similar results have been reported in several other social referencing studies as well (e.g., L. J. Carver & Vaccaro, 2007;Hertenstein & Campos, 2001;Hornik et al, 1987). Furthermore, Klinnert et al's (1986) study (discussed in section 1a), which did not reveal a negativity bias in 12-month-olds' social referencing behavior, did suggest such a bias in infants' affect.…”
Section: B the Negativity Bias In Emotional Contagionsupporting
confidence: 85%
“…necklace glomeruli | pheromone | olfaction | PTSD | social referencing C hildren, including infants, use their parents' emotions to guide their behavior and learn about safety and danger (1)(2)(3)(4). The infant's ability to regulate behavior in novel situations using the caregiver's emotional expression is known as social referencing and occurs in humans and nonhuman primates (1).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The infant's ability to regulate behavior in novel situations using the caregiver's emotional expression is known as social referencing and occurs in humans and nonhuman primates (1). Although parental physical presence itself or particular cues indicating parental presence, such as voice, touch, or smell typically signal safety for the child, infants are especially responsive to the caregiver's communication during threats (3)(4)(5). This social learning is critical for enhancing survival through an adaptation to the environment but also provides transmission of pathological fears, such as occurs in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or in specific phobias (3)(4)(5)(6)(7).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation