2013
DOI: 10.1075/lllt.34.13ch9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

9. A corpus approach to studying structural convergence in task-based Spanish L2 interactions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
0
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
3
22
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…There is a tendency for a form to precede (i.e., prime) another similar form, as has been shown in the case of perseveration or the “birds of a feather” phenomenon, by which the use of null and overt subject pronouns tends to favor the repeated use of null and overt pronouns, respectively (e.g., Cameron & Flores‐Ferrán, ). This phenomenon is also similar to the notion of structural convergence (e.g., Collentine & Collentine, ). In the current study, the most frequent forms in the data, the PF, the MF, the PI, and lexical futures were all coded as possible preceding forms.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 78%
“…There is a tendency for a form to precede (i.e., prime) another similar form, as has been shown in the case of perseveration or the “birds of a feather” phenomenon, by which the use of null and overt subject pronouns tends to favor the repeated use of null and overt pronouns, respectively (e.g., Cameron & Flores‐Ferrán, ). This phenomenon is also similar to the notion of structural convergence (e.g., Collentine & Collentine, ). In the current study, the most frequent forms in the data, the PF, the MF, the PI, and lexical futures were all coded as possible preceding forms.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 78%
“…In the picture and map tasks, speakers needed to achieve common task goals (e.g., complete story or map), and thus may have increased their tendency to reuse the same structures, which resulted in the high overall rates of structural alignment (65-69% of all repeated utterances) in both tasks. This result is in line with evidence shown by McDonough (in review), where communicative tasks involving less turnby-turn interaction between interlocutors (which presumably are less demanding in terms of speakers' need to interact to achieve a shared goal) are less likely to elicit structural alignment, compared to highly interactive tasks engaging both interlocutors (see also Collentine & Collentine, 2013;Reitter & Moore, 2014).…”
supporting
confidence: 90%
“…After primes and targets were coded, two proportion scores per interlocutor pair were calculated, following similar research (e.g., Collentine & Collentine, 2013;Gries, 2005). The first score was the prime→target score, computed by dividing the number of sequences where a prime was followed by a target over the total number of repeated utterances produced in each dialogue (across all structures combined), separately in each task.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Marsden & Mackey 2013;Polio 2015;Porte 2012; different research and service initiatives at, for example, the Center for Applied Linguistics, and as part of professional organizations, such as the American Association for Applied Linguistics), special issues of journals on methodology (e.g. the Disciplinary Dialogues section on replication in the Journal of Second Language Writing, 2012, 21,3), books on research processes (Matsuda & Silva 2005), a few landmark replications (as examples, Muñoz 1995;Rebuschat & Williams 2006), methods textbooks (Dörnyei 2007;Gass & Mackey 2007;Mackey & Gass 2005;Porte 2010;Richards, Ross & Seedhouse 2011) as well as an increasing tradition of research syntheses and meta-analyses (Norris & Ortega 2006;Plonsky & Oswald 2012). As Gass (2015) points out, there are statements from leading journals in the field.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%