2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.07.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A big fish or a small pond? Framing effects in percentages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Descriptive and presentational aspects of product attributes also affect choice. A nonexhaustive list includes positive or negative framing (Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, 1998), the unit of magnitude (Lembregts & Pandelaere, 2013), the denominator for basing a percentage (Li & Chapman, 2013), compatibility (Maciejovsky & Budescu, 2013), salience (Enax, Krajbich, & Weber, 2016), attentional attraction (Mackenzie, 1986), the emotional content of presentations (Lee, Amir, & Ariely, 2009), and ease of perceptual processing (Shah & Oppenheimer, 2007). Large experimental literatures also demonstrate context effects specific to choices that involve risk (e.g., investments; for discussion, see Vlaev, Chater, & Stewart, 2008) or partitioned prices (Greenleaf, Johnson, Morwitz, & Shalev, 2016).…”
Section: Related Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Descriptive and presentational aspects of product attributes also affect choice. A nonexhaustive list includes positive or negative framing (Levin, Schneider, & Gaeth, 1998), the unit of magnitude (Lembregts & Pandelaere, 2013), the denominator for basing a percentage (Li & Chapman, 2013), compatibility (Maciejovsky & Budescu, 2013), salience (Enax, Krajbich, & Weber, 2016), attentional attraction (Mackenzie, 1986), the emotional content of presentations (Lee, Amir, & Ariely, 2009), and ease of perceptual processing (Shah & Oppenheimer, 2007). Large experimental literatures also demonstrate context effects specific to choices that involve risk (e.g., investments; for discussion, see Vlaev, Chater, & Stewart, 2008) or partitioned prices (Greenleaf, Johnson, Morwitz, & Shalev, 2016).…”
Section: Related Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As noted above, only a few studies provide empirical evidence of consumers' responses to 100% claims; these responses, as might be expected, are generally positive (Berry et al, 2017; Li & Chapman, 2009, 2013). However, works in this vein have focused on claims in which the perfection implied by the term 100% is compatible with the associated product attribute (e.g., a vaccine that is “100% effective” against the virus strains it targets, meaning that it completely prevents infection with those viruses; Li & Chapman, 2009).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Previous research suggests that people attribute excessive weight to the advantages offered by “perfect” attributes ( perfection premium ; Isaac & Spangenberg, 2021), which may motivate marketers to use perfection to advance their products (Spurgin, 2003). In the context of product claims, a study has shown that the use of the term “100%” can successfully elicit a perfection premium; consumers evaluated coffee more favorably when one of its ingredients was described as “100% organic” (compared with 25%, 40%, 50%, 80%, or 95% organic; Li & Chapman, 2013).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies of framing demonstrate that people routinely accept information as it is given rather than actively processes it with multiple frames (Kühberger, 1998;Levin, Schneider & Gaeth, 1998;Li & Chapman, 2013;Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). Framing effects can reverse preferences.…”
Section: Trade-upgrade Framing Effectsmentioning
confidence: 99%