2011
DOI: 10.1002/acp.1793
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Comparison of Adult Witnesses' Suggestibility Across Various Types of Leading Questions

Abstract: The current study directly compared witnesses' susceptibility to suggestion across various structures of misleading interview questions. We examined four question structures that varied on numerous dimensions; whether they narrowed the response option to yes or no, whether they included highly specific detail about the witnessed event and whether they presumed the information being suggested to be true. Susceptibility to misinformation was measured by witnesses' responses to the initial interview questions and… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
50
1
3

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(55 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
1
50
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, the leading information within the question may have triggered memory recall in children and prompted verbose responses. As such questions can significantly compromise the accuracy of children's and adults' accounts (Ceci et al, 2002;Sharman & Powell, 2012) and are particularly harmful to the accounts of Aboriginal witnesses (Eades, 2008(Eades, , 2013, we strongly discourage the use of suggestive questions with Aboriginal children. This finding, however, points to the idea of including nonsuggestive cues in open-ended questions with Aboriginal children.…”
Section: Substantive Phasementioning
confidence: 93%
“…Therefore, the leading information within the question may have triggered memory recall in children and prompted verbose responses. As such questions can significantly compromise the accuracy of children's and adults' accounts (Ceci et al, 2002;Sharman & Powell, 2012) and are particularly harmful to the accounts of Aboriginal witnesses (Eades, 2008(Eades, , 2013, we strongly discourage the use of suggestive questions with Aboriginal children. This finding, however, points to the idea of including nonsuggestive cues in open-ended questions with Aboriginal children.…”
Section: Substantive Phasementioning
confidence: 93%
“…Leading questions were alternated with misleading questions in order to decrease children's suspicion of the interviewer. For diversity, leading and misleading questions also alternated between formats: (1) forced‐choice (e.g., ‘Did the Deakin Activities happen on a Thursday or a Friday?’); (2) specific cued‐recall (e.g., ‘What colour was the lady's bag?’); (3) closed yes/no (e.g., ‘Did the koala puppet have a friend?’); and (4) closed tag (e.g., ‘You ran around in a circle, didn't you?’) (Sharman & Powell, ). Each interview was concluded by thanking the child for being helpful.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…open questions), not least the dangers of leading, closed questions (e.g. Sharman & Powell, 2012). Current 'gold standard' interview techniques, such as the Cognitive Interview (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992) and the National Institute for Child Health and Development Protocol (NICHD; Lamb, Orbach, Hershkowitz, Esplin, & Horowitz, 2007) endorse the use of open-ended questions with compliant witnesses (for overview, see Vrij, Hope, & Fisher, 2014).…”
Section: Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%