2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2018.09.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A comparison of tests for quantifying sensory eye dominance

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
19
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 63 publications
1
19
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We should also stress that this potential is achieved with most precision when ocular weights are estimated as described here (not when the balance point is measured in conventional usage) and when data are collected with a dual-presentation same-different paradigm (not with the method of adjustment). Other variants of the interocular combination paradigm were recently proposed that also seek to estimate a balance point (see Bossi et al, 2017), but they require observers to report judgments of perceived contrast or direction of motion and do not produce data that can be used to estimate ocular weights.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We should also stress that this potential is achieved with most precision when ocular weights are estimated as described here (not when the balance point is measured in conventional usage) and when data are collected with a dual-presentation same-different paradigm (not with the method of adjustment). Other variants of the interocular combination paradigm were recently proposed that also seek to estimate a balance point (see Bossi et al, 2017), but they require observers to report judgments of perceived contrast or direction of motion and do not produce data that can be used to estimate ocular weights.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other tests also not considered in this article are based on binocular rivalry paradigms of various sorts (e.g., Dieter, Sy, & Blake, 2017;Y. Ding, Naber, Gayet, Van der Stigchel, & Paffen, 2018;Handa et al, 2006;Handa, Shimizu, Uozato, Shoji, & Ishikawa, 2012;Valle-Incla´n, Blanco, Soto, & Leiro´s, 2008;Xu, He, & Ooi, 2010; see also Bossi, Hamm, Dahlmann-Noor, & Dakin, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to previous studies, the depth of imbalanced interocular suppression has been quantified with laboratory-based psychophysical procedures, including global coherent motion task (Mansouri, Thompson, & Hess, 2008), binocular phase combination task (Ding, Klein, & Levi, 2013;Huang et al, 2009;Kwon et al, 2015;, dichoptic EDTRS task (Kwon et al, 2015), binocular rivalry task (Ooi & He, 2001), optokinetic nystagmus paradigm (Wen et al, 2018), dichoptic noise masking paradigm (Zhou et al, 2018), and so on (see a comparison of tests in Bossi, Hamm, Dahlmann-Noor, & Dakin, 2018). These techniques, although precise and quantitative, are not friendly for children in the clinical setting because they normally involve the use of complicated devices and/or take a long time to complete.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whether aging affects different binocular functions similarly or differently remains an open question. Although there is evidence that sensory eye dominance estimated with interocular contrast differences in binocular combination, binocular rivalry, and stereopsis was correlated with each other (Han, He, & Ooi, 2018;Xu, He, & Ooi, 2011), many other studies have suggested that different binocular functions may have different neural underpinnings, even for the same measurement but tested with different methods (Bossi, Hamm, Dahlmann-Noor, & Dakin, 2018;Bosten et al, 2015;Garnham & Sloper, 2006;Ukai et al, 2003;Wang et al, 2018). For example, Garnham and Sloper (2006) showed that the decline of stereoacuity measured with TNO chart was the greatest in some of his older subjects, as opposed to stereoacuity measured with Titmus, Frisby near, and Frisby-Davis distance stereo tests.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%