2013
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00676
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A computational model to investigate assumptions in the headturn preference procedure

Abstract: In this paper we use a computational model to investigate four assumptions that are tacitly present in interpreting the results of studies on infants' speech processing abilities using the Headturn Preference Procedure (HPP): (1) behavioral differences originate in different processing; (2) processing involves some form of recognition; (3) words are segmented from connected speech; and (4) differences between infants should not affect overall results. In addition, we investigate the impact of two potentially i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, and somewhat closer to the skill discussed here, Gerken, Balcomb and Minton (2011) document that infants look longer when they are listening to stimuli from which a rule can be discovered compared to very similar sequences where no such rule is present. Put more generally, in the context of word segmentation from native speech, infants' attention may be better predicted by other factors, such as the characteristics of the stimuli (see also Aslin, 2007;Bergmann, ten Bosch, Fikkert & Boves, 2013).…”
Section: Revising Mainstream Assumptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, and somewhat closer to the skill discussed here, Gerken, Balcomb and Minton (2011) document that infants look longer when they are listening to stimuli from which a rule can be discovered compared to very similar sequences where no such rule is present. Put more generally, in the context of word segmentation from native speech, infants' attention may be better predicted by other factors, such as the characteristics of the stimuli (see also Aslin, 2007;Bergmann, ten Bosch, Fikkert & Boves, 2013).…”
Section: Revising Mainstream Assumptionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, although words are often explored in novelty/familiarity paradigms either in isolation (e.g., Hallé and de Boysson-Bardies, 1994 , 1996 ; Vihman et al, 2004 ; Swingley, 2005 ) or in passages of sentences containing target words (e.g., Jusczyk and Aslin, 1995 ; Jusczyk et al, 1999 ; Bortfeld et al, 2005 ; Singh, 2008 ; Singh et al, 2012 ; DePaolis et al, 2014 ), there are few methodological examinations of familiarity and novelty as they apply specifically to the developing lexicon. One exception is a computational model of factors affecting word segmentation in AHPP experiments ( Bergmann et al, 2013 ). Another is a study ( DePaolis et al, 2013 ) that found that 12-month-old infants’ preference for non-words was linearly related to the number of consonants each infant produced that were featured in the test stimuli; effectively, the infants showed either familiarity or novelty, based upon the extent of their previous practice with the test stimuli (see DePaolis et al, 2011 and Majorano et al, 2014 for similar differences in infant preference based upon their babbling patterns).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the whole database, only 6 studies delay the test phase and/or use exposures in the child's home; and none of them were part of the final analyses given our constraint of including only studies that measured two age groups with the same stimuli. Further, since infants are usually familiarized and tested in close succession, it might be sufficient for them to perform a sort of phonetic pattern matching, which need not employ symbolic representations and/or their long-term memory [24]. We hope future work will help address that this important gap in the current literature.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%