2008
DOI: 10.1037/a0013396
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A context-based theory of recency and contiguity in free recall.

Abstract: We present a new model of free recall based on Howard and Kahana's (2002) temporal context model and Usher and McClelland's (2001) leaky-accumulator decision model. In this model, contextual drift gives rise to both short-term and long-term recency effects, and contextual retrieval gives rise to short-term and long-term contiguity effects, Recall decisions are controlled by a race between competitive leaky-accumulators. The model captures the dynamics of immediate, delayed, and continual distractor free recall… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

25
542
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 348 publications
(567 citation statements)
references
References 110 publications
(276 reference statements)
25
542
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our interpretation of our data may go some way towards overcoming the difficulties that unitary accounts of IFR (e.g., Brown et al, 2007; Howard & Kahana, 2002;Polyn, et al, 2009;Sederberg, et al, 2008;Tan & Ward, 2000) have in explaining the IFR of short lists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Our interpretation of our data may go some way towards overcoming the difficulties that unitary accounts of IFR (e.g., Brown et al, 2007; Howard & Kahana, 2002;Polyn, et al, 2009;Sederberg, et al, 2008;Tan & Ward, 2000) have in explaining the IFR of short lists.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Retrieval is often assumed to be both self-propagating and self-limiting (e.g., Roediger, 1973Roediger, , 1974, such that the recall of one list item can facilitate the recall of the next (e.g., Howard & Kahana, 1999; Kahana, 1996; Nairne, Ceo & Reysen, 2007; see also Lohnas & Kahana, 2014), but can also cause output interference (e.g., Beaman, 2002; Bunting, Cowan & Saults, 2006, Cowan, Saults, Elliott & Moreno, 2002 Nairne et al, 2007;Oberauer, 2003;Tan & Ward, 2007). It may be that the "ISR-like" recall of short lists is an effective strategy to recall many of the words from short lists, but that when only one or two responses are required, participants favor the greater certainty of accessing only the most recent items.Our interpretation of our data may go some way towards overcoming the difficulties that unitary accounts of IFR (e.g., Brown et al, 2007; Howard & Kahana, 2002;Polyn, et al, 2009;Sederberg, et al, 2008;Tan & Ward, 2000) have in explaining the IFR of short lists.Unitary accounts of IFR tend to predict extended recency effects, and so to date they have been found wanting in explaining participants' tendencies to initiate the recall of short lists with the first list item and to continue to recall in an "ISR-like" manner. However, our data suggest that, even in the IFR of very short lists, participants prefer to initiate recall with one of the last list items if they only have to recall one or two list items.…”
mentioning
confidence: 64%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The simplified cued recall model we proposed to explain source memory judgments was, in turn, based on a samplingand-recovery model of recall (Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1981;Diller et al, 2001) that was itself motivated by the dynamics of inter-retrieval times and stopping times. The temporal context model (Howard & Kahana, 2002) has also been extended to account for the dynamics of recall using a competing accumulator approach similar to the one we have used to explain recognition (Sederberg, Howard, & Kahana, 2008;Polyn, Norman, & Kahana, 2009). These extensions revealed, among other things, the importance of semantic associations between successively recalled items predicting inter-response latencies, which would not necessarily have been apparent otherwise.…”
Section: Other Aspects Of Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%