This is the draft version of the paper.This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Copyright and reuse: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ publications@city.ac.uk
Permanent repository link:
City Research OnlineBeginning at the beginning:Recall order and the number of words to be recalled When participants are asked to recall a short list of words in any order that they like, they tend to initiate recall with the first list item and proceed in forwards order, even when this is not a task requirement. The current research examined whether this tendency might be influenced by varying the number of items that are to be recalled. In three experiments, participants were presented with short lists of between 4 and 6 words and instructed to recall 1, 2, 3 or all of the items from the lists. Data were collected using immediate free recall (IFR, Experiment 1), immediate serial recall (ISR, Experiment 2) and a variant of ISR that we call ISR-free (Experiment 3), in which participants had to recall words in their correct serial positions but were free to output the words in any order. For all three tasks, the tendency to begin recall with the first list item occurred only when participants were required to recall as many items from the list as they could. When participants were asked to recall only one or two items, they tended to initiate recall with end-of-list items. It is argued that these findings show for the first time a manipulation that eliminates the initial tendency to recall in forward order, provide some support for recency-based accounts of IFR and help explain differences between single-response and multiple-response immediate memory tasks.(232 words)In recent years, there has been a growing belief that much could be gained by the theoretical integration of two widely used and highly influential immediate memory tasks, immediate serial recall (ISR) and immediate free recall (IFR) (e.g., Anderson, Bothell, Lebiere, & Matessa, 1998;Bhatarah, Ward & Tan, 2006 Brown, Chater & Neath, 2008; Brown, Neath & Chater, 2007;Farrell, 2012;Grenfell-Essam & Ward, 2012; Grossberg & Pearson, 2008; Hurlstone, Hitch & Baddeley, 2014; Kahana, 2012; Klein, Addis & Kahana, 2005;Ward, Tan & Grenfell-Essam, 2010). In these tasks, participants are presented with lists of words and at the end of each list, they are either free to recall as many of the list items as possible in any order that they like (IFR) or they are required to recall as many items as possible in exactly the same serial order as that in which they were presented (ISR).One reason to believe that such integration is possible is the observation that participants tend to output their recalls in forwards serial order, even in IFR tasks (e.g., Beaman & Mor...