1996
DOI: 10.1121/1.417338
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A developmental study of vowel perception from brief synthetic consonant–vowel syllables

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to assess the perceptual role of brief synthetic consonant-vowel syllables as cues for vowel perception in children and adults. Nine types of consonant-vowel syllables comprised of the stops [b d g] followed by the vowels [i a u] were synthesized. Stimuli were generated with durations of 10, 30, or 46 ms, and with or without formant transition motion. Eight children at each of five age levels (5, 6, 7, 9, and 11 years) and a control group of eight adults were trained to identify e… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
21
0
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
2
21
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…At the nonspeech level, this is in line with (1) physiological animal studies showing that neural coding for temporal aspects of the stimulus reaches maturity later than neural coding for frequency selectivity (Eggermont, 1996), and with (2) behavioral auditory studies in humans providing evidence for a more prolonged development of the sensitivity for temporal than for nontemporal auditory cues (Hartley, Wright, Hogan, & Moore, 2000), even after accounting for the effect of procedure-related skills (Dawes & Bishop, 2008). At the speech level, it coincides with behavioral speech perception studies demonstrating that the identification of stop consonants is not yet mature by the age of 11 (Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Johnson, 2000;Krause, 1982;Simon & Fourcin, 1978;Medina, Hoonhorst, Bogliotti, & Serniclaes, 2010), whereas the identification of vowels does only slightly, though not significantly, improves towards adolescence (Pursell, Swanson, Hedrick, & Nabelek, 2002;Ohde, Haley, & McMahon, 1996;Johnson, 2000, but see Walley and Flege, 1999). Further elaboration on this topic is needed, but the indication that perception of sounds with temporal versus nontemporal cues follows different maturational trajectories in both normal and dyslexic readers, may have practical implications with regard to auditory temporal training programs.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…At the nonspeech level, this is in line with (1) physiological animal studies showing that neural coding for temporal aspects of the stimulus reaches maturity later than neural coding for frequency selectivity (Eggermont, 1996), and with (2) behavioral auditory studies in humans providing evidence for a more prolonged development of the sensitivity for temporal than for nontemporal auditory cues (Hartley, Wright, Hogan, & Moore, 2000), even after accounting for the effect of procedure-related skills (Dawes & Bishop, 2008). At the speech level, it coincides with behavioral speech perception studies demonstrating that the identification of stop consonants is not yet mature by the age of 11 (Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Johnson, 2000;Krause, 1982;Simon & Fourcin, 1978;Medina, Hoonhorst, Bogliotti, & Serniclaes, 2010), whereas the identification of vowels does only slightly, though not significantly, improves towards adolescence (Pursell, Swanson, Hedrick, & Nabelek, 2002;Ohde, Haley, & McMahon, 1996;Johnson, 2000, but see Walley and Flege, 1999). Further elaboration on this topic is needed, but the indication that perception of sounds with temporal versus nontemporal cues follows different maturational trajectories in both normal and dyslexic readers, may have practical implications with regard to auditory temporal training programs.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…However, contrary to this view, some prior developmental results might be interpreted to support a formant target model (Sussman, 2001). Ohde et al (1996) speculated that "vowels may be apprehended as unique perceptual units early in development," because even young infants can perform speaker normalization. Infants have demonstrated an ability to discriminate vowel signals regardless of age, gender, etc.…”
Section: B Development Of Vowel Perception: Formant Onsets and Formamentioning
confidence: 96%
“…of the speaker and can even link acoustic to facial cues by four months of age (Kuhl, 1979(Kuhl, , 1983. Only a few other studies have been conducted specifically on the development of vowel perception (Parnell and Amerman, 1978;Murphy et al, 1989;Ohde et al, 1996;Ohde and Haley, 1997;Malech and Ohde, 2003). Of these studies, none have examined potential differences in cue weighting of vowels in children compared to adults.…”
Section: B Development Of Vowel Perception: Formant Onsets and Formamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…O conhecimento acumulado sobre percepção de fala ressalta também o desenvolvimento como importante aspecto nesta competência perceptual (Montgomery & Clarkson, 1997;Ohde, Haley, & McMahon, 1996). Já ao nascimento o bebê possui capacidade de detectar contrastes consonantais, por exemplo, sendo esta habilidade não apenas inata, mas característica do sistema auditivo dos mamíferos.…”
Section: Percepção De Fala No Primeiro Ano De Vidaunclassified