2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.01.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A lateralization of function approach to sex differences in spatial ability: A reexamination

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
22
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
5
22
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Firstly, reaction times were faster for back-facing than front-facing pictures (see also e.g., Arzy et al, 2007; Mohr et al, 2010, in press; Thakkar and Park, 2010; Cooper and Mohr, 2012) indicating that participants performed mental transformations. This finding is in line with previous reports regarding the mental rotation of objects (Shepard and Metzler, 1971; Wohlschläger and Wohlschläger, 1998), body parts (Cooper and Shepard, 1975; Parsons, 1987; Bonda et al, 1995; Petit et al, 2003; Seurinck et al, 2004), and perspective taking tasks (e.g., Kaiser et al, 2008; Rilea, 2008) which report longer reaction times when the position of a stimulus (or own current body position) does not match the position of the target stimulus. Moreover, mental perspective transformations for female figures were faster than those for male figures, an advantage that was specific to back-facing figures, supporting previous observations (see Mohr et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Firstly, reaction times were faster for back-facing than front-facing pictures (see also e.g., Arzy et al, 2007; Mohr et al, 2010, in press; Thakkar and Park, 2010; Cooper and Mohr, 2012) indicating that participants performed mental transformations. This finding is in line with previous reports regarding the mental rotation of objects (Shepard and Metzler, 1971; Wohlschläger and Wohlschläger, 1998), body parts (Cooper and Shepard, 1975; Parsons, 1987; Bonda et al, 1995; Petit et al, 2003; Seurinck et al, 2004), and perspective taking tasks (e.g., Kaiser et al, 2008; Rilea, 2008) which report longer reaction times when the position of a stimulus (or own current body position) does not match the position of the target stimulus. Moreover, mental perspective transformations for female figures were faster than those for male figures, an advantage that was specific to back-facing figures, supporting previous observations (see Mohr et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…The present findings, however, showed the opposite. Previous studies comparing the sexes on comparable versions of the 3PP‐task showed no sex difference (Mohr et al , 2006; Rilea, 2008), pointed to a male advantage (Ofte, 2002), in particular when of young age (Ofte & Hugdahl, 2002), did not test for sex differences (Arzy et al , 2006, Expt 2; Bailey et al , 2007; Blanke et al , 2005; Easton et al , 2009), or tested only male participants (Arzy et al , 2006, Expt 1, 2007; Zacks et al , 1999). So, although most relevant studies either did not find or did not examine sex differences, the two that did point to a male advantage in the 3PP‐task; in line with the present findings.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…These findings are not surprising in light of literature supporting bilateral distribution of complex cognitive functions in females. Females show greater bilateral activation and interhemispheric connectivity than males in language functions, assessment of beauty, and spatial processing (Bitan, Lifshitz, Breznitz, & Booth, 2010; Gur et al, 2000; McGlone, 1980; Rilea, 2008). Additionally, recent data from Ingalhalikar et al (2014) examining diffusion-based structural connectomes suggest that female brains are optimized for interhemispheric (rather than intrahemispheric) communication.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%