“…Of the 14 studies included in this review, 13 (93%) reported negative associations between maternal pre‐pregnancy smoking, smoking during pregnancy or exposure to smoke and childhood language outcomes (Alati et al, 2008; Eriksen et al, 2012; Gilman et al, 2008; Heinonen et al, 2011; Hernandez‐Martinez et al, 2017; Hsieh et al, 2008; Huijbregts et al, 2006; Julvez et al, 2007; Lee et al, 2019; MacArthur et al, 2001; Mohamed et al, 2018; Neumann et al, 2019; Polanska et al, 2017) (Figure 2, Table 2). Differences in study design and reporting methods make it difficult to directly compare effects between studies but six of the fourteen studies reviewed (43%) report highly significant effects (Alati et al, 2008; Gilman et al, 2008; Hernandez‐Martinez et al, 2017; Hsieh et al, 2008; Huijbregts et al, 2006; MacArthur et al, 2001) ( p ≤ 0.001) and seven found marginal effects (0.001 < p < 0.05) (Eriksen et al, 2012; Heinonen et al, 2011; Julvez et al, 2007; Lee et al, 2019; Mohamed et al, 2018; Neumann et al, 2019; Polanska et al, 2017) (Figure 2, Table 2). Although most studies found consistent associations, their conclusions differed; five concluded that the effects of smoking on child language could be explained by indirect effects, primarily socioeconomic in nature (Alati et al, 2008; Eriksen et al, 2012; Gilman et al, 2008; Huijbregts et al, 2006; MacArthur et al, 2001) (Figure 2, Table 2).…”