1985
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.1985.tb00977.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A longitudinal study of the development of the object concept

Abstract: Object concept development was studied longitudinally in a group of 24 infants from 12 weeks until achievement of Stage 6. Infants were tested between 12 and 28 weeks on four visual tracking tasks: a simple, unoccluded tracking task and three others, each involving a different spatial relationship in mid‐track between the object and an occluder (a screen, tunnel, or platform). Competence on manual search tasks was monitored from the age at which reaching first appeared. Understanding of the same three spatial … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2011
2011

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Infants reach not for the smallest or closest of two objects but for segmented surfaces whose boundaries are distinguished from others by separation in depth or motion (von Hofsten & Spelke, 1985). If these segmentation conditions are violated, say by placing a reached-for object directly onto a larger one, reaching is disrupted (Wishart & Bower, 1985). ' The salience of potentially usable information that is made explicit by input descriptions lies in its availability and use at the action program level-hence, in what the subject is trying to do, not in how exhaustively it models things in the world.…”
Section: ~ -mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Infants reach not for the smallest or closest of two objects but for segmented surfaces whose boundaries are distinguished from others by separation in depth or motion (von Hofsten & Spelke, 1985). If these segmentation conditions are violated, say by placing a reached-for object directly onto a larger one, reaching is disrupted (Wishart & Bower, 1985). ' The salience of potentially usable information that is made explicit by input descriptions lies in its availability and use at the action program level-hence, in what the subject is trying to do, not in how exhaustively it models things in the world.…”
Section: ~ -mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Children with Down's syndrome pass the Piagetian tasks (Cicchetti & Sroufe, 1976) albeit taking additional months to do so (see also, Gunn & Berry, this volume). There is research (Wishart & Bower, 1985) indicating that training can accelerate success on the cognitive development tasks although there is not a full body of work on this point. All of these results suggest that development in infancy is heavily canalizedor, in other words, that it follows a shared, species-specific path (McCall, 1981).…”
Section: Fostering Development In Infancymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Visually-directed search might be a more appropriate behavioural index of understanding in this group of children at these young age levels, although visual tracking behaviours can be much more time-consuming to analyse and more difficult to interpret than manual search patterns (see e.g. Wishart and Bower, 1985).…”
Section: Performance On Object Concept Tasks At Older Agesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although it is known that repeated exposure to object concept tasks can lead to accelerated development (Wishart and Bower, 1985) and although clearly this effect was greater in normally developing children than in children with Down's syndrome (see Table 1), it is nevertheless important to register that many of these children with Down's syndrome managed to reach the levels of understanding required to succeed on this task more or less on time, no mean achievement given the very strict criteria adopted in this study and their inherent cognitive disadvantage.…”
Section: Performance On Object Concept Tasks At Older Agesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation