2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.024
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A mechanism for spatial perception on human skin

Abstract: Our perception of where touch occurs on our skin shapes our interactions with the world. Most accounts of cutaneous localisation emphasise spatial transformations from a skin-based reference frame into body-centred and external egocentric coordinates. We investigated another possible method of tactile localisation based on an intrinsic perception of 'skin space'. The arrangement of cutaneous receptive fields (RFs) could allow one to track a stimulus as it moves across the skin, similarly to the way animals nav… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
28
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, the dominant source of error during path integration is noise that accumulates with distance travelled (Stangl et al, 2020). Interesting, similar errors are found for path integration in the tactile domain (Fardo et al, 2018;Moscatelli et al, 2014), suggesting that they may involve multilateration as well.…”
Section: Is Multilateration a General Spatial Computation?mentioning
confidence: 59%
“…Further, the dominant source of error during path integration is noise that accumulates with distance travelled (Stangl et al, 2020). Interesting, similar errors are found for path integration in the tactile domain (Fardo et al, 2018;Moscatelli et al, 2014), suggesting that they may involve multilateration as well.…”
Section: Is Multilateration a General Spatial Computation?mentioning
confidence: 59%
“…S-space may be understood as a tactile analogue of a visual field: it allows computations of spatial relations between various points on the body on the basis of topological information stemming from neighboring relations between receptive fields. Cheng and Haggard (2018) suggest that such a space may be sufficient for body spatiality to arise.…”
Section: Multisensory Integration Hypothesis Vs Thementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, the emergence of RHI depends on the degree of discrepancy between locations of sensory signals in external space, but the degree of discrepancy on the body is deemed irrelevant (or rather, assumed to be equal to 0). Recently, the term "skin space" ("S-space") has been coined (Haggard, Cheng, Beck, & Fardo, 2017;Cheng & Haggard, 2018;Fardo, Beck, Cheng, & Haggard, 2018) to describe a spatial representation allowing tactile localization on the surface of the skin. S-space may be understood as a tactile analogue of a visual field: it allows computations of spatial relations between various points on the body on the basis of topological information stemming from neighboring relations between receptive fields.…”
Section: Multisensory Integration Hypothesis Vs Thementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Different cues contribute to the perception of motion in touch, including the spatiotemporal pattern of local deformation, as for example the one generated by the tip of a pencil moving across the skin, high-frequency skin vibrations occurring during slip motion, and the gross skin deformation, such as the stretch generated by a shear force applied to the fingertip [32,11,13,18,29,1]. The role of the first two motion cues, local indentation and vibrations, was deeply investigated in recent studies [5,9,15,31,23,8]. Here, we evaluate the role of shear deformation for the discrimination of speed.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The indentation produced by traceable surface elements, like moving raised dots or ridges, provide a salient motion cue in touch [32,5,15]. Recently, a model based on the spatiotemporal pattern of skin deformation reproduced the tactile afferent signals quite accurately [29].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%