2018
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1087-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A meta-analysis of contingent-capture effects

Abstract: The present meta-analyses investigated the widely used contingent-capture protocol. Contingent-capture theory postulates that only top-down matching stimuli capture attention. Evidence comes from the contingent-capture protocol, in which participants search for a predefined target stimulus preceded by a spatial cue. The cue is typically uninformative of the target's position but either presented at target position (valid condition) or away from the target (invalid condition). The common finding is that seeming… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
42
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 104 publications
3
42
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, this effect is difficult to replicate (Eimer & Kiss, 2010;Irons et al 2012) and, recently, Schoeberl, Goller and Ansorge (2019) failed to find evidence that feature priming could explain contingent capture. Additionally, in a metaanalysis, Büsel, Voracek, and Ansorge (2018;see also Lamy & Kristjánsson, 2013) found no evidence that contingent capture could be explained through intertrial priming. As such, it is unlikely the results from the present experiments were due to intertrial priming.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…However, this effect is difficult to replicate (Eimer & Kiss, 2010;Irons et al 2012) and, recently, Schoeberl, Goller and Ansorge (2019) failed to find evidence that feature priming could explain contingent capture. Additionally, in a metaanalysis, Büsel, Voracek, and Ansorge (2018;see also Lamy & Kristjánsson, 2013) found no evidence that contingent capture could be explained through intertrial priming. As such, it is unlikely the results from the present experiments were due to intertrial priming.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…When the cues do not have the searched-for target feature (i.e., in the case of a nonmatching cue), the cues do not lead to a validity effect. This pattern of results is labeled the contingent-capture effect and has been replicated many times under various conditions (e.g., Carmel & Lamy, 2014;Eimer, Kiss, Press, & Sauter, 2009;Goller & Ansorge, 2015; for a review, see Büsel, Voracek, & Ansorge, 2018). It is regarded as evidence that people are able to attend to certain nonspatial features in a top-down way, because cues that have the target's critical searched-for feature capture attention as if the observers were willingly attending to the corresponding features.…”
mentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Stimuli can draw our attention in two different ways ( Egeth and Yantis, 1997 ; Wolfe et al, 2003 ; Connor et al, 2004 ; Weichselbaum et al, 2018 ): in an intentional, goal-directed way (top–down/endogenous) or in an automatic, stimulus-driven way (bottom–up/exogenous). In top–down capture, only a stimulus matching the goals and/or search intentions of the observer will capture attention; for example, when looking for a tomato in the supermarket, only red and round objects will be selected for further processing (e.g., Duncan and Humphreys, 1989 ; Folk et al, 1992 ; Büsel et al, 2018 ). Irrelevant stimuli, however, are suppressed and ignored (e.g., inattentional blindness; Mack and Rock, 1998 ; Eitam et al, 2013 ; Horstmann and Ansorge, 2016 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%