2014
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00064
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A “Misfit” Theory of Spontaneous Conscious Odor Perception (MITSCOP): reflections on the role and function of odor memory in everyday life

Abstract: Our senses have developed as an answer to the world we live in (Gibson, 1966) and so have the forms of memory that accompany them. All senses serve different purposes and do so in different ways. In vision, where orientation and object recognition are important, memory is strongly linked to identification. In olfaction, the guardian of vital functions such as breathing and food ingestion, perhaps the most important (and least noticed and researched) role of odor memory is to help us not to notice the well-know… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
61
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 90 publications
(64 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
2
61
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Nevertheless, for such memories to arise it is necessary that the food experience is sufficiently similar to the original experience. Even slight changes in a common food, such as the addition of another spice or a change in texture, will readily be consciously detected and lead to pleasant surprise or may disturb the intimate memories and evoke feelings of disappointment and dissatisfaction, instead of the upcoming pleasant reminiscences raised when the food is conform to our expectations (Köster et al, 2014). Strangely enough the role of memory is almost always neglected in food-related consumer research, although it is probably much more important than the first impression experiences that are commonly investigated.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 53%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Nevertheless, for such memories to arise it is necessary that the food experience is sufficiently similar to the original experience. Even slight changes in a common food, such as the addition of another spice or a change in texture, will readily be consciously detected and lead to pleasant surprise or may disturb the intimate memories and evoke feelings of disappointment and dissatisfaction, instead of the upcoming pleasant reminiscences raised when the food is conform to our expectations (Köster et al, 2014). Strangely enough the role of memory is almost always neglected in food-related consumer research, although it is probably much more important than the first impression experiences that are commonly investigated.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…Nevertheless, these influences are probably much weaker than the ones that are based on experiences in earlier eating situations and carry the emotional content of these earlier occasions usually with them. In fact, there is ample evidence that we do not remember the food we ate earlier with precision, but are immediately reminded of the earlier situation in which we ate it (its ambiance or the company we ate it with) or we note deviations from it as a surprise and warning: "attention, not encountered before in the same or a similar situation" (Köster et al, 2014;Morin-Audebrand et al, 2012). In a preliminary study on the "memorability" of foods -the spontaneous occurrence of food memories in non-eating situations -the present authors also obtained indications that the emotions related to the eating situations and not the properties of the food itself raise such spontaneous memories, even though the latter may have contributed to the emotional colour of the former.…”
Section: Effects Of Expectations and Memory On Emotions And Moodmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Interoception is closely connected to the fundamental workings of the chemical senses which, as opposed to the other senses, act as gate keepers of what is 'outside' and what is 'inside' the body. Interoceptive food studies might bring studies of the chemical senses forward by focusing more on the ecological roles of these senses, as opposed to the tendency of looking for similarities between vision and olfaction, for example [28 ]. Finally, it is worth noting that neuroimaging studies of other pleasures than eating (sex, drugs, monetary gains) have shown that many of the same neural networks are active during experience of the different types of pleasures, suggesting that humans are endowed with a unitary pleasure system.…”
Section: Food Rewardmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Koster, Moller, and Mojet (2014) published data showing that olfactory-induced influences (i.e., observed behaviour change associated with an odour) affect us even when we are not consciously aware of either the presence of the odour or its effect on our behaviour. In other words, we may not be able to articulate or even identify the odour-related influences behind the way in which we appreciate or judge a wine.…”
Section: Accepted Manuscriptmentioning
confidence: 99%