2013
DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00161
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A neurobiology of learning beyond the declarative non-declarative distinction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
7
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In memory research, since episodes are typically characterized as unique configurations of stimuli, episodic memory can be considered one-trial learning of stimulus-stimulus relations (e.g., Ortu & Vaidya 2013). As mentioned earlier, the hippocampus is critically involved in selection of stimulus-stimulus relations and receives concurrent neuromodulation by multiple neurotransmitter systems, including noradrenaline.…”
Section: Daniele Ortumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In memory research, since episodes are typically characterized as unique configurations of stimuli, episodic memory can be considered one-trial learning of stimulus-stimulus relations (e.g., Ortu & Vaidya 2013). As mentioned earlier, the hippocampus is critically involved in selection of stimulus-stimulus relations and receives concurrent neuromodulation by multiple neurotransmitter systems, including noradrenaline.…”
Section: Daniele Ortumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sidman et al (1968) related their DMTS findings with Molaison to those of Milner (1962, as cited by Sidman et al, 1968) with a mirror‐tracing task in which the participant had to draw a line with a pencil between the borders of a reflection of a star without crossing its borders (Corkin, 2013; Kandel, 2018; Ortu & Vaidya, 2013; Squire, 2009). With enough training, Molaison learned both procedures.…”
Section: Sidman's Experiments With Molaison As the Participantmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One-trial encoding, relation formation, and long-term representation of relations in a flexible format are the defining properties of episodic memory (Cohen and Eichenbaum, 1993 ; O'Reilly and Rudy, 2001 ; Henke, 2010 ). Because memories formed from subliminal stimuli lived up to this computational definition (Reber et al, 2012 ; Duss et al, 2014 ; Ruch et al, 2016 ) it must be assumed that episodic memory formation may proceed with and without conscious awareness (Henke, 2010 ; Dew and Cabeza, 2011 ; Hannula and Greene, 2012 ; Olsen et al, 2012 ; Ortu and Vaidya, 2013 ). Such a notion is all the more surprising as the formation and retrieval of episodic memory, mediated by the hippocampus, was long thought to depend on conscious awareness (e.g., Moscovitch, 1995 ; Squire and Zola, 1996 ; Tulving, 2002 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This view was inspired by the finding that hippocampal lesions impaired declarative (conscious) memory for facts and episodes but spared non-declarative (non-conscious) forms of memory such as motor skill learning, priming, and conditioning (Squire and Zola, 1996 ). Studies using implicit or indirect tests to examine non-conscious forms of episodic memory (e.g., Rose et al, 2002 ; Henke et al, 2003 ; Greene et al, 2007 ; Hannula and Ranganath, 2009 ; Duss et al, 2014 ; Ryals et al, 2015 ; Rosenthal et al, 2016 ) revealed, however, that the function of the hippocampus does not depend on consciousness but on the type of processing involved at learning: the hippocampus mediates the rapid relational encoding and storage of both consciously and unconsciously apprehended information (for reviews see Henke, 2010 ; Dew and Cabeza, 2011 ; Hannula and Greene, 2012 ; Olsen et al, 2012 ; Ortu and Vaidya, 2013 ). Both conscious and unconscious relational memories were stored for long-term (Ruch et al, 2016 ) within the hippocampal memory space (Züst et al, 2015 ) with overlapping conscious and unconscious relational memories interacting with each other (Henke et al, 2013 ; Züst et al, 2015 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%