2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104084
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A ‘no’ with a trace of ‘yes’: A mouse-tracking study of negative sentence processing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
16
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
6
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It could, for example, be that since self-paced reading is a less natural way of reading than on-screen reading in rapid visual serial presentation (as used, e.g., in comparable EEG studies), the burden on working memory of remembering a complex picture, forming (pragmatic expectations) and reading text in a self-paced manner is too onerous a task. In fact, in a recent study employing mousetracking, Darley et al (2020) found that processing sentences against a visual context becomes more costly the higher the number of pragmatically-licensed sentence continuations, such that, in their study, higher numbers of possible continuations led to a decrease in task accuracy, an increase in the speed of responses, as well as higher degrees of attraction to foil responses in the measured mouse trajectories. The authors concluded that " [.…”
Section: Processing Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It could, for example, be that since self-paced reading is a less natural way of reading than on-screen reading in rapid visual serial presentation (as used, e.g., in comparable EEG studies), the burden on working memory of remembering a complex picture, forming (pragmatic expectations) and reading text in a self-paced manner is too onerous a task. In fact, in a recent study employing mousetracking, Darley et al (2020) found that processing sentences against a visual context becomes more costly the higher the number of pragmatically-licensed sentence continuations, such that, in their study, higher numbers of possible continuations led to a decrease in task accuracy, an increase in the speed of responses, as well as higher degrees of attraction to foil responses in the measured mouse trajectories. The authors concluded that " [.…”
Section: Processing Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…As far as linguistic prediction is concerned, studies have shown that material can be pre-activated at different levels of linguistic representation, from phonologically- and lexically-driven pre-activation to pre-activation derived from syntactic and semantic cues (e.g., Mani and Huettig, 2012 ; Boudewyn et al, 2015 ; Urbach et al, 2015 ; Kuperberg and Jaeger, 2016 ), although some of these findings—particularly with respect to phonological prediction—have failed to replicate in more recent studies (e.g., Nieuwland et al, 2020 ; see also Kuperberg and Jaeger, 2016 for a detailed discussion of the different sorts of prediction potentially involved in predictive language processing). As for pragmatics, many studies have provided evidence that high-level semantic and pragmatic prediction occurs while people process language, from the processing of negation (e.g., Nieuwland, 2016 ; Haase et al, 2019 ; see also Scappini et al, 2015 ; Darley et al, 2020 ) to the processing of sentences containing potentially pragmatic cues such as the scalar quantifier some (e.g., Nieuwland et al, 2010 ; Augurzky et al, 2019 ). Although the bulk of the evidence stems from neurolinguistic studies, other studies have drawn on behavioral methods such as eye-tracking (e.g., Degen and Tanenhaus, 2016 ; Scholman et al, 2017 ; Huang and Snedeker, 2018 ) and self-paced reading (e.g., Bicknell and Rohde, 2009 ; Bergen and Grodner, 2012 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It stands to reason that inferring what states of affairs are truth-makers when considering the result of composing a sentence meaning with identity would typically involve less computational resources compared to when the composition is with negation, because the latter operation requires extra steps of inference. Beyond the reading time differences reported in Matthews et al (1980), further evidence for this cost of computing the actual state of affairs is reported in Darley, Kent, and Kazanina (2020) and shows up indirectly in individual difference research, such as Reuter et al (2018), andMargolin (2015); also indirectly in McLeod et al (1978) and Matthews et al (1980), who find that participants with better visual abilities are more inclined to adopt the one-step procedure.…”
Section: Two Procedures For Verifying Negative Sentencesmentioning
confidence: 86%
“…Further support for the composite view can be found in evidence from a wide range of paradigms beyond SPV tasks, which indicate active representation of the positive argument of negation. These include probe recognition (Hasson & Glucksberg, 2006;Kaup et al, 2007a), ERP (Dudschig et al, 2019;Fischler et al, 1983), visual world (Nordmeyer & Frank, 2014;Orenes et al, 2014) and mouse-tracking (Dale & Duran, 2011;Darley et al, 2020).…”
Section: Composite Vs Interpretive Theories Of Negation Comprehensionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, artificial contexts used in research could explain the negation effect. Nevertheless, there are some studies that have found that negative sentences take longer to be processed than affirmative ones even when they are presented in a supportive context (see Darley et al, 2020;Orenes et al, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%