2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A “picture” of children's potential for learning: Looking into strategy changes and working memory by dynamic testing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

4
55
1
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
4
55
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Oral explanations of problem solving also reflect metacognitive skills (Efklides, 2006) and do not necessarily parallel children's problem-solving processes, particularly if they are young (Ericsson & Simon, 1984;Veenman, 2015). It seems, therefore, that children can (partially) construct an answer but (still) operate at different levels of verbal and behavioral strategy-use, being able to offer a verbal account only after training (Resing et al, 2012) [see also, Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Loehr, and Miller (2015) for related findings in the early numeracy domain].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Oral explanations of problem solving also reflect metacognitive skills (Efklides, 2006) and do not necessarily parallel children's problem-solving processes, particularly if they are young (Ericsson & Simon, 1984;Veenman, 2015). It seems, therefore, that children can (partially) construct an answer but (still) operate at different levels of verbal and behavioral strategy-use, being able to offer a verbal account only after training (Resing et al, 2012) [see also, Rittle-Johnson, Fyfe, Loehr, and Miller (2015) for related findings in the early numeracy domain].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the present study we explicitly focused on the differential involvement of verbal and visual-spatial working-memory components, to examine their possible role in respect of changes in analogical reasoning over time. We thought it important to examine these components separately with a working-memory assessment that made sufficient storage and processing demands (Alloway, 2007) and which would help us explore their influence on analogical reasoning (Resing, Xenidou-Dervou, Steijn & Elliott, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For children aged five years and older, the literature has shown the benefit of combining observations of behavioural solution strategies with their immediate descriptions of how they sought to solve the problem. The value of this approach has been shown in studies in various domains, such as arithmetic (Siegler & Stern, 1998), reading (Farrington-Flint, Coyne, Stiller, & Heath, 2008, and inductive reasoning (Resing, Xenidou-Dervou, Steijn, & Elliott, 2012;Stevenson, Hickendorff, Resing, Heiser, & de Boeck, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We expected that children would only show transfer once they had become good strategic problem solvers (Siegler, 2006). Optimal performance on inductive reasoning tasks is strongly influenced by strategy-use (Siegler & Svetina, 2002), and training has been shown to increase children's ability to explain the changes that occur within the tasks (Resing et al, 2012). Therefore, those children who were able to explain their problem-solving behaviour at the end of the study period were expected to construct the reversal transfer tasks in a particularly effective manner They are likely to have gained a good understanding of the underlying principles of the figural analogies (Harpaz-Itay, Kaniel, & Ben-Amram, 2006;Perkins, 1992), which should enable them to apply this knowledge to the transfer task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%