2016
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00318.2015
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A quantitative confidence signal detection model: 1. Fitting psychometric functions

Abstract: Yi Y, Merfeld DM. A quantitative confidence signal detection model: 1. Fitting psychometric functions. J Neurophysiol 115: 1932J Neurophysiol 115: -1945J Neurophysiol 115: , 2016. First published January 13, 2016; doi:10.1152/jn.00318.2015.-Perceptual thresholds are commonly assayed in the laboratory and clinic. When precision and accuracy are required, thresholds are quantified by fitting a psychometric function to forced-choice data. The primary shortcoming of this approach is that it typically requires 10… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

1
23
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 14 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
1
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another clue that something is particularly unique about size is the strength of the SWI relative to other weight illusions. For example, in considering the SWI alongside the MWI, which is the next most studied weight illusion, evidence suggests that the SWI is considerably stronger (Buckingham & Goodale, 2013;Buckingham, Michelakakis, & Cole, 2016). In the Buckingham and Goodale's (2013) study mentioned earlier, participants were tested on both illusions, in that they lifted two sets of large and small stimuli, one apparently made from polystyrene and one from aluminum.…”
Section: Size Exerts a Strong Influence On Weight Expectations And Pementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another clue that something is particularly unique about size is the strength of the SWI relative to other weight illusions. For example, in considering the SWI alongside the MWI, which is the next most studied weight illusion, evidence suggests that the SWI is considerably stronger (Buckingham & Goodale, 2013;Buckingham, Michelakakis, & Cole, 2016). In the Buckingham and Goodale's (2013) study mentioned earlier, participants were tested on both illusions, in that they lifted two sets of large and small stimuli, one apparently made from polystyrene and one from aluminum.…”
Section: Size Exerts a Strong Influence On Weight Expectations And Pementioning
confidence: 99%
“…More specifically, using forced-choice tasks in which observation duration is constrained but response time is not, we investigated 1) whether perceptual choice and confidence mechanisms include evidence accumulation and 2) whether such evidence accumulation is terminated by decision bounds. In short, two accumulator models, bounded DDM with "absorbing" bounds (Ratcliff and McKoon 2008) and unbounded DDM without decision bounds, were augmented to include confidence probability judgments (Yi and Merfeld 2016), and then these models were compared with empiric SVV binary choice (i.e., is the visual scene tilted left or right?) and confidence (i.e., what is the probability that the choice is correct?)…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is a general consensus that confidence reflects the state of the decision variables, although it is still debated whether decision and confidence involve a single-or double-stage process (Murphy et al 2015;Navajas et al 2016;Pleskac and Busemeyer 2010;Rahnev et al 2015;van den Berg et al 2016;Yu et al 2015). Yi and Merfeld recently proposed a psychophysical model explaining the correlations between confidence and perceptual precision (Yi and Merfeld 2016). Here we combine this earlier confidence model with a DDM.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations