1994
DOI: 10.1007/bf02692155
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A sensitive period for learning about food

Abstract: It is proposed here that there is a sensitive period in the first two to three years of life during which humans acquire a basic knowledge of what foods are safe to eat. In support of this, it is shown that willingness to eat a wide variety of foods is greatest between the ages of one and two years, and then declines to low levels by age four. These data also show that children who are introduced to solids unusually late have a narrower diet breadth throughout childhood, perhaps because the duration of the sen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

13
178
0
6

Year Published

2003
2003
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 314 publications
(197 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
13
178
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…A significant body of literature documents the proclivity of humans and other mammals to select food from the environment through modeling the food selection of others (Birch, 1980;Galef, 1989;Rozin & Kennel, 1983;Rozin, 1976). It has been hypothesized that this behavior is innate and adaptive in guiding the selection of 'safe,' non-poisonous items from the environment (Cashdan, 1994). The present study extends these findings by indicating that children are also more likely to indiscriminately trust adult testimony when the stimulus is a food compared to a non-food.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…A significant body of literature documents the proclivity of humans and other mammals to select food from the environment through modeling the food selection of others (Birch, 1980;Galef, 1989;Rozin & Kennel, 1983;Rozin, 1976). It has been hypothesized that this behavior is innate and adaptive in guiding the selection of 'safe,' non-poisonous items from the environment (Cashdan, 1994). The present study extends these findings by indicating that children are also more likely to indiscriminately trust adult testimony when the stimulus is a food compared to a non-food.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…As might be expected, these attitudes (as indicated, for example, by both breadth of diet and incidence of accidental poisonings) vary in a predictable way throughout the life cycle, with curiosity holding sway during the first two years of life and neophobia reaching its zenith around age 5 (Cashdan, 1994;1998). A similar pattern was observed in a study of free-living Japanese macaques: when candy was introduced to the group, only 10% of adults ate candy on initial presentation, whereas 50% of monkeys less than three years old did; a year after the initial introduction, 100% of 1-yearolds continued to eat candy, whereas only 51% of older females and 32% of older males did (Itani, 1958).…”
mentioning
confidence: 67%
“…Mothers reported how often the child had eaten the food in the past 4 weeks ( Table 1). To reduce food neophobia, 29 the experimenter ate 1 Oreo cookie off of the plate and said, "I'm going to have one, too. Mmm, this is really good.…”
Section: Eah Protocolmentioning
confidence: 99%