2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.01.006
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A sentence to remember: Instructed language switching in sentence production

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

7
38
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(46 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
7
38
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In particular, they demonstrate that syntactic structure has robust effects on the size of language switch costs, in general agreement with proposals of greater automaticity for specific types of switches (reflecting structural principles; Myers-Scotton, 1993; 2002; 2005; 2006), and with other recent studies that arrived at the same conclusion using different methods and via examination of a very limited set of structural alternations (Declerck & Philipp, 2015a; Dussias, 2003; Fairchild & Van Hell, in press; Rossi et al, submitted). Unique to our study was the finding that grammaticality modulated switch costs not only in production speed, but also in total failures of language control (intrusion errors), and in diverse speaker groups (including both young and older bilinguals).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In particular, they demonstrate that syntactic structure has robust effects on the size of language switch costs, in general agreement with proposals of greater automaticity for specific types of switches (reflecting structural principles; Myers-Scotton, 1993; 2002; 2005; 2006), and with other recent studies that arrived at the same conclusion using different methods and via examination of a very limited set of structural alternations (Declerck & Philipp, 2015a; Dussias, 2003; Fairchild & Van Hell, in press; Rossi et al, submitted). Unique to our study was the finding that grammaticality modulated switch costs not only in production speed, but also in total failures of language control (intrusion errors), and in diverse speaker groups (including both young and older bilinguals).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…Importantly, grammatical switches were not cost free; though intrusions did not reveal switch costs in grammatical paragraphs (they revealed an unexpected effect in the opposite direction), bilinguals’ whole paragraph reading times were significantly slower for high- than for low-switch paragraphs. Additionally, intrusion errors exhibited robust part of speech effects, consistent with theories that posit distinct control mechanisms for retrieval of targets that assign thematic roles versus those that do not (Myers-Scotton, 2006), the finding of robust part of speech effects in naturally occurring intrusion errors (Poulisse, 1999), and inconsistent with the absence of part of speech effects in other recently reported experimental paradigms (Declerck & Philipp, 2015a). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It might also be useful to investigate similarity in phonological form between translation equivalents (cognate effects; Gollan et al, 2014), part of speech effects (Gollan & Goldrick, 2016), and syntactic influences on the ease with which bilinguals can switch. It is known, for example, that switches are easier when the participating languages share word order in a sentence (Dussias; Declerck & Philipp, 2015b), and that certain combinatorial patterns are preferred over others in spontaneous code-switches (Tamargo, Valdés Kroff, & Dussias, 2016). Such factors might be differentially sensitive to language control deficits in AD.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, in mixed-language blocks, bilinguals name pictures slower even on non-switch trials relative to single-language blocks; these mixing costs reflect the need to actively maintain response readiness in both languages (Declerck & Philipp, 2015). In addition, bilinguals sometimes name pictures faster in the non-dominant than the dominant language, either only on switch trials (leading to asymmetric switch costs ; Meuter & Allport, 1999) or on both switch and non-switch trials (leading to reversed dominance effects ; Christoffels, Firk, & Schiller, 2007; Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Gollan & Ferreira, 2009; Verhoef, Roelofs, & Chwilla, 2009; for review, see Declerck & Philipp, 2015). Both patterns suggest top-down control operating either via activation (boosting) of the non-dominant language, or inhibition (active suppression) of the dominant language, or both.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%