1969
DOI: 10.1016/s0065-2504(08)60259-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A Simulation Model of Animal Movement Patterns

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
75
0

Year Published

1979
1979
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 123 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
3
75
0
Order By: Relevance
“…When these results are compared with some of the few data available for the relationship between sample size and the reliability of home range estimates, the guideline of 100·200 10-cations is higher. For example, Siniff & Jessen (1969) found that in a simulation of animal movements based on 400-800 locations, 100 points were sufficient for a reliable estimate of movement patterns. For coyotes (Canis latrans), it has been suggested that about 40-50 "independent" locations [assumptions of independence are rarely if ever addressed (Anderson, 1982); see Schoener (1981) for a method for testing independence] were sufficient to estimate reliably home range size (Bowen, 1982;Messier & Barrette, 1982;Smith et al, 1981), whereas for wolves (c. lupus), 35·120 locations were needed (Fritts & Mech, 1981).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…When these results are compared with some of the few data available for the relationship between sample size and the reliability of home range estimates, the guideline of 100·200 10-cations is higher. For example, Siniff & Jessen (1969) found that in a simulation of animal movements based on 400-800 locations, 100 points were sufficient for a reliable estimate of movement patterns. For coyotes (Canis latrans), it has been suggested that about 40-50 "independent" locations [assumptions of independence are rarely if ever addressed (Anderson, 1982); see Schoener (1981) for a method for testing independence] were sufficient to estimate reliably home range size (Bowen, 1982;Messier & Barrette, 1982;Smith et al, 1981), whereas for wolves (c. lupus), 35·120 locations were needed (Fritts & Mech, 1981).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As in many other areas of research, data concerning space use and movement patterns often are easy to gather (or appear to be easy to collect) but difficult to interpret. Theoretical approaches are numerous (Anderson, 1982;Cooper, 1978;Ford & Krumme, 1979;Hayne, 1949;Heezen & Tester, 1967;Jennrich& Turner, 1969;Koeppl, Slade, & Hoffmann, 1975Metzgar & Sheldon, 1974;Mohr, 1947;Mohr & Stumpf, 1966;Rasmussen, 1980;Schoener, 1981;Siniff & Jessen, 1969;Stickel, 1954;van Winkle, 1975), but the "correct" method of analysis is closely related to the questions being asked and to the method used to generate the data; there does not appear to be a right or a wrong way to analyze space use or movement patterns.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since most animals have a tendency to move forwards (persistence), CRWs have been frequently used to model animal paths in various contexts (e.g. Siniff & Jessen 1969;Skellam 1973;Kareiva & Shigesada 1983;Bovet & Benhamou 1988;Turchin 1998).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Numerous studies of foraging in fishes have reported patterns of preferences relating to particular habitat types at various scales (Hobson 1975, Cowen 1986, Grossman 1986). Many studies, predominantly theoretical, have attempted to explain foraging-path patterns by relating the distribution and size of preferred habitat patches to patterns of space use during foraging (Siniff & Jessen 1969, Covich 1976. In particular, optimal foraging theory regards the distribution of prey into microhabitat patches and the attendant trade-offs associated with travelling between or remaining within these patches as a major factor affecting the shape of foraging paths (Schoener 1971, Norberg 1977.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In particular, optimal foraging theory regards the distribution of prey into microhabitat patches and the attendant trade-offs associated with travelling between or remaining within these patches as a major factor affecting the shape of foraging paths (Schoener 1971, Norberg 1977. Theoretical predictions based on the distribution of microhabitats indicate that directed (nonoverlapping) paths are best for searching discrete microhabitats in small patches, whereas a convoluted (overlapping) foraging path most efficiently searches locally uniform microhabitats (Siniff & Jessen 1969, Weihs & Webb 1983.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%