1986
DOI: 10.1002/sce.3730700412
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A study of consistency in the use of students' conceptual frameworks across different task contexts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

10
90
0
20

Year Published

1996
1996
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 199 publications
(120 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
10
90
0
20
Order By: Relevance
“…This group selects either a Newtonian or a non-Newtonian answer based on context. This further supports research showing that some students can hold both a correct and an alternate model at the same time and apply them based on the contextual features of the situation [10][11][12]14]. Note that in thep 3 vector for the Spring 2006 pretest threecluster model, the largest component is neither the correct answer nor the dominant misconception, but is a third component.…”
Section: Clustering Of Course Datasupporting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This group selects either a Newtonian or a non-Newtonian answer based on context. This further supports research showing that some students can hold both a correct and an alternate model at the same time and apply them based on the contextual features of the situation [10][11][12]14]. Note that in thep 3 vector for the Spring 2006 pretest threecluster model, the largest component is neither the correct answer nor the dominant misconception, but is a third component.…”
Section: Clustering Of Course Datasupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Substantial variation in the consistency of the application of alternate frameworks and scientifically correct frameworks was found with students often using both the correct model and an alternate model in different situations [10][11][12]. The consistency of student reasoning (either correct or incorrect) was found to increase with additional instruction [12][13][14] and to depend on the familiarity of the context of the problems [15].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Their empirical work showed that undergraduates believed that it was possible for an animal to acquire a physical property in its lifetime provided that it had a useful function or was a good fit with environmental conditions (Ware and Gelman 2014, p. 234). Like Kargbo et al's (1980) and Clough and Driver's (1986) studies, student ideas about inheritance were impacted by item features, although in this case aspects of animal trait functions.…”
Section: Genetics Educationmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…In a longitudinal study of the consistency of 12-through 16-year-old conceptions about inheritance of acquired characteristics and other scientific phenomena, Clough and Driver (1986) found that task context was most significant for students who had yet to develop normative scientific understanding. Interviews of students with lower knowledge levels revealed conceptions about acquired characteristics that displayed less consistency across parallel tasks (e.g., taillessness in mice, athletic ability in humans, and rough skin caused by gardening in humans) compared to interviews of students with higher knowledge levels.…”
Section: Genetics Educationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…molecules of water are blue (e.g. Brook, Briggs, & Driver, 1984;Meheut & Chomat, 1990;Novick & Nussbaum, 1978); (b) gas' distribution is not homogeneous, when an action is exerted on gas (Benson, Wittrock, & Baur, 1993;Méheut, Larcher, Chomat, & Barboux, 1988); (c) gases have no mass (Séré, 1985;Stavy, 1988), (d) warm air rises inside a closed container (Séré, 1985); (e) gases without movements do not exert any force (Clough & Driver, 1986;Séré, 1985); (f) there is only one effect associated to one cause (Viennot, 2001); (g) severable variables on gas do not change simultaneously (de Berg, 1992;Meheut & Chomat, 1990;Séré, 1985).…”
Section: Teaching Sequencementioning
confidence: 99%