2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104710
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A systematic review and meta-analysis of attentional bias toward food in individuals with overweight and obesity

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

6
40
2

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
6
40
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In a more recent systematic review (Hagan, Alasmar, Exum, Chinn, & Forbush, 2020), the effects of different attentional bias paradigms were taken into account by conducting separate meta-analyses per task type (dot probe, emotional stroop, eye-tracking, ERPs). Studies using the dot probe task, eye tracking measures and ERPs were also separately aggregated based on the attentional component measured (e.g., stimulus presentations of ≤200 ms and ≥500 ms for the dot probe task) as a means of distinguishing between early and late attentional processing.…”
Section: The Relationship Between Food Ab and Obesitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a more recent systematic review (Hagan, Alasmar, Exum, Chinn, & Forbush, 2020), the effects of different attentional bias paradigms were taken into account by conducting separate meta-analyses per task type (dot probe, emotional stroop, eye-tracking, ERPs). Studies using the dot probe task, eye tracking measures and ERPs were also separately aggregated based on the attentional component measured (e.g., stimulus presentations of ≤200 ms and ≥500 ms for the dot probe task) as a means of distinguishing between early and late attentional processing.…”
Section: The Relationship Between Food Ab and Obesitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two experiments sought to explore the existence of a mnemonic bias towards highcalorie food stimuli. Attentional biases to high-calorie food stimuli have previously been reported (Hagan et al, 2020;Mogg et al, 1998) and there are also mnemonic biases towards remembering the consumption and location (de Vries et al, 2020;New et al, 2007) of high-calorie foods. A bias towards high-calorie food stimuli could also facilitate the incentive value of food cues, thus resulting in overconsumption (Berridge et al, 2010).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…First, as previously stated, cues with high incentive value are thought to be particularly salient and attractive (Berridge et al, 2010). This has often been used as a theoretical explanation for attentional biases towards food stimuli particularly in overweight and obese populations described earlier (Hagan et al, 2020). Similar reasoning is used to explain data from neuroimaging studies suggesting these populations show elevated reactivity to food cues in a variety of different brain regions (Boswell & Kober, 2016;Jastreboff et al, 2014;Leyton & Vezina, 2013;Stice, Spoor, Bohon, Veldhuizen, & Small, 2008).…”
Section: Is There a Memory Bias For High-calorie Food Images?mentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For instance, though much work has shown memory deficits are associated with obesity, and that memory for recent eating moderates future eating, to our knowledge, no studies have examined how participant BMI interacts with this latter pattern. Similarly, while attentional biases to food and food cues are observed in participants with obesity (Hagan, Alasmar, Exum, Chinn, & Forbush, 2020;Werthmann, Jansen, & Roefs, 2015), it remains untested whether mnemonic biases for these items are also observed and more or less pronounced in those with obesity. How memory of eating differs in normal versus overweight and obese populations may be particularly interesting, given serum levels of leptin and ghrelin differ in these populations (Klok, Jakobsdottir, & Drent, 2007) and are also implicated in mnemonic processes (Suarez et al, 2019).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%