2002
DOI: 10.1121/1.4777987
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acoustic and perceptual evidence of complete neutralization of word-final tonal specification in Japanese

Abstract: This study investigates the extent to which Japanese lexical pitch-accent distinction is neutralized in word-final position. Native speakers of Tokyo Japanese produced minimal word pairs differing in final accent status. Words were produced both in isolation and in a sentential context, where neutralization would not be expected due to following tonal specification. Examination of pitch patterns on relevant moras revealed a clear distinction between accent-opposed pairs produced in context but no such differen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The acoustic features of the different pitch accent patterns of the disyllabic triplets used in the current dichotic test were analyzed using Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2013). The acoustic features included here have been shown to be relevant perceptual cues, as reviewed above (e.g., Hasegawa & Hata, 1992; Maniwa, 2002; Sugiyama, 2006, 2008, 2014). First, to track F0 trajectories and determine contour direction for different pitch accent patterns (primarily between H*L and LH*/LH), F0 values at 0%, 25%, 50%, 75% and 100% of the first and second syllables of the disyllabic words, as well as the average F0 values for each syllable, were measured.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The acoustic features of the different pitch accent patterns of the disyllabic triplets used in the current dichotic test were analyzed using Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2013). The acoustic features included here have been shown to be relevant perceptual cues, as reviewed above (e.g., Hasegawa & Hata, 1992; Maniwa, 2002; Sugiyama, 2006, 2008, 2014). First, to track F0 trajectories and determine contour direction for different pitch accent patterns (primarily between H*L and LH*/LH), F0 values at 0%, 25%, 50%, 75% and 100% of the first and second syllables of the disyllabic words, as well as the average F0 values for each syllable, were measured.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Each word was recorded four times by a female linguistically-trained native speaker of Tokyo Japanese (aged 32) in a sound-attenuated recording booth in the Language and Brain Lab at Simon Fraser University, using Presonus Digital Audio 24 B27/96K Firewire recording interface and a Shure KSM 109 microphone. Each word was followed by a monosyllabic particle, including が (-ga), を (-wo), に (-ni), と (-to), and の (-no), to provide a phrasal context for the native speaker to naturally and accurately produce the distinctions among the pitch accent patterns, especially those between the accented and unaccented patterns (Maniwa, 2002).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For example, the two-mora hashi can carry three different lexical meanings as “chopsticks” (H*L: initial accented mora with a high pitch followed by a mora with a lower pitch), “bridge” (LH*: initial low pitch followed by an accented mora with a higher pitch), or “edge” (LH: initial low pitch followed by a higher pitch with neither mora accented). These accent categories are primarily cued by F0 rise/fall, though F0 peak, duration, and amplitude serve as secondary cues (Beckman, 1986; Hasegawa & Hata, 1992; Maniwa, 2002).…”
Section: Perception Of Japanese Pitch Accent By L1 and L2 Listenersmentioning
confidence: 99%