“…Behavioral L2 studies revealed that higher L2 proficiency facilitated L2 prediction based on morphosyntactic associations (e.g., Lew-Williams and Fernald, 2010;Dussias et al, 2013;Sagarra et al, 2021;Henry et al, 2022;see Ito and Pickering, 2021, for a review; and see Mitsugi, 2020, for lack of proficiency effects), phonosemantic associations (Perdomo and Kaan, 2021, for bin 5), and morphophonological associations (Sagarra and Casillas, 2018). Higher L2 proficiency also benefited L2 morphosyntactic processing (see 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1141174 Frontiers in Psychology 14 frontiersin.org Kirova and Camacho, 2021, for a review), as well as L2 morphological processing (Kimppa et al, 2019), L2 word activation (Berghoff et al, 2021), and L2 phonological processing (Jun and Oh, 2000;White et al, 2015;Konishi et al, 2018;Maddah and Reiterer, 2018). Neurocognitive L2 studies indicated that higher L2 proficiency facilitated L2 morphosyntactic processing (see Alemán Bañón et al, 2018, for a review) and shaped the brain (Pliatsikas et al, 2020), allowing learners to activate the same brain areas as monolinguals (Vingerhoets et al, 2003).…”