2001
DOI: 10.1121/1.1357814
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acoustic-phonetic features for the automatic classification of fricatives

Abstract: In this article, the acoustic-phonetic characteristics of the American English fricative consonants are investigated from the automatic classification standpoint. The features studied in the literature are evaluated and new features are proposed. To test the value of the extracted features, a statistically guided, knowledge-based, acoustic-phonetic system for the automatic classification of fricatives in speaker-independent continuous speech is proposed. The system uses an auditory-based front-end processing s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…COG can be used to distinguish sibilant fricatives with different places of articulation (/s z/ vs. /ʃ ʒ/); nonsibilants (/f v θ ð/) tend to have a broad and flat distribution of energy with overall higher spectral peaks relative to sibilants, but with COGs medial to the alveolar (/s z/) and postalveolar (/ʃ ʒ/) sibilants (e.g., Ali, Van der Spiegel, & Mueller, 2001;Forrest et al, 1988;Hughes & Halle, 1956;Jongman et al, 2000). Critically, the characteristic COG of a fricative can vary according to the phonetic context (e.g., the value for /s/ is lower before rounded vowels such as /u/ than before unrounded vowel such as /i/; Jongman et al, 2000;Soli, 1981;Yu, 2019), speech style (e.g., /s/ has a lower value in casual than in careful speech; Maniwa, Jongman, & Wade, 2009), talker gender (e.g., /s/ is lower on average for male than for female speakers of American English; Flipsen et al, 1999;Fuchs & Toda, 2010), and even across individual talkers of the same gender (e.g., Newman et al, 2001).…”
Section: Cue Calibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…COG can be used to distinguish sibilant fricatives with different places of articulation (/s z/ vs. /ʃ ʒ/); nonsibilants (/f v θ ð/) tend to have a broad and flat distribution of energy with overall higher spectral peaks relative to sibilants, but with COGs medial to the alveolar (/s z/) and postalveolar (/ʃ ʒ/) sibilants (e.g., Ali, Van der Spiegel, & Mueller, 2001;Forrest et al, 1988;Hughes & Halle, 1956;Jongman et al, 2000). Critically, the characteristic COG of a fricative can vary according to the phonetic context (e.g., the value for /s/ is lower before rounded vowels such as /u/ than before unrounded vowel such as /i/; Jongman et al, 2000;Soli, 1981;Yu, 2019), speech style (e.g., /s/ has a lower value in casual than in careful speech; Maniwa, Jongman, & Wade, 2009), talker gender (e.g., /s/ is lower on average for male than for female speakers of American English; Flipsen et al, 1999;Fuchs & Toda, 2010), and even across individual talkers of the same gender (e.g., Newman et al, 2001).…”
Section: Cue Calibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…COG can be used to distinguish sibilant fricatives with different places of articulation (/s z/ vs. /ʃ ʒ/); nonsibilants (/f v θ ð/) tend to have a broad and flat distribution of energy with overall higher spectral peaks relative to sibilants, but with COGs medial to the alveolar (/s z/) and postalveolar (/ʃʒ/) sibilants (e.g., Ali, Van der Spiegel, & Mueller, 2001;Forrest et al, 1988;Hughes &Halle,1956;Jongman et al, 2000). Critically, the characteristic COG of a fricative can vary according to the phonetic context (e.g., the value for /s/ is lower before rounded vowels such as /u/ than before unrounded vowel such as /i/; Jongman et al, 2000;Soli, 1981;Y u,2019), speech style (e.g., /s/ has a lower value in casual than in careful speech; Maniwa, Jongman, & Wade, 2009), talker gender (e.g., /s/ is lower on average for male than for female speakers of American English; Flipsen et al, 1999;Fuchs & Toda, 2010), and even across individual talkers of the same gender (e.g., Newman et al, 2001).…”
Section: Cue Calibrationmentioning
confidence: 99%