1972
DOI: 10.3758/bf03328891
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acquisition of correct choices and value judgments in binary choice learning with differential rewards

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
9
0

Year Published

1972
1972
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
3
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants who initially found the value-learning task more challenging may have been oriented more toward value during the free-recall test, thus producing a positive relationship between trials-to-criterion and the effect of value free recall, opposite to what was observed with the effect of value on lexical decision. This indirect evidence of two distinct value-learning mechanisms may be related to similar dissociations in probabilistic value-learning strategies reported by others (Humphreys et al, 1968 ; Allen and Estes, 1972 ; Estes, 1972 ; Medin, 1972a ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Participants who initially found the value-learning task more challenging may have been oriented more toward value during the free-recall test, thus producing a positive relationship between trials-to-criterion and the effect of value free recall, opposite to what was observed with the effect of value on lexical decision. This indirect evidence of two distinct value-learning mechanisms may be related to similar dissociations in probabilistic value-learning strategies reported by others (Humphreys et al, 1968 ; Allen and Estes, 1972 ; Estes, 1972 ; Medin, 1972a ).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 80%
“…Participants were first presented with words in a two-alternative choice value-learning task, in which they learned, by trial-and-error with feedback, that half of the words led to a high-value reward and half of the words led to a low-value reward (also used by Madan and Spetch, 2012 ). This value-learning task is similar to previous reward-learning procedures used by Estes and others (e.g., Pubols, 1960 ; Estes, 1962 , 1966 , 1972 ; Humphreys et al, 1968 ; Allen and Estes, 1972 ; Medin, 1972a , b ) as well several more recent reward-learning studies (e.g., Johnsrude et al, 1999 , 2000 ; Frank et al, 2004 , 2006 ; Bayley et al, 2005 ; Pessiglione et al, 2006 ; Valentin and O’Doherty, 2009 ; Voon et al, 2010 ; Gradin et al, 2011 ). Participants were then presented with an unrewarded lexical decision task, in which words from the value-learning task were shown again.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…In aseparate attempt to obtain direct observations of the states of learning assumed by the reward magnitude model, Allen & Estes (1972) required Ss to estimate the value of the stimulus alternative they chose during reward magnitude learning. Allen and Estes found that, contrary to the assumptions of the general model, the rate of acquisition of correet value estimates was systematieally related to reward value and , further, that there was a signifieant improvement in choosing the higher valued alternative of a stimulus pair before the first evidence appeared that a S had learned either of the reward values.…”
Section: Stimulus and Reward Combinationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conditioning studies have presented neutral pictures with aversive outcomes, particularly sounds or electric shocks, and tested for subsequent changes in memory due to these associations (Mather & Knight, 2008;Sakaki et al, 2014;Dunsmoor et al, 2015). Convergently, studies of rewardmemory effects that have used procedures where rewards are conditioned to be associated with reward values have found reward-related subsequent memory effects (Estes, 1966;Allen & Estes, 1972;Madan & Spetch, 2012;Chakravarty et al, 2019).…”
Section: Affective Bleedmentioning
confidence: 99%