2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.018
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Adaptation of timing behavior to a regular change in criterion

Abstract: This study examined how operant behavior adapted to an abrupt but regular change in the timing of reinforcement. Pigeons were trained on a fixed interval (FI) 15-s schedule of reinforcement during half of each experimental session, and on an FI 45-s (Experiment 1), FI 60-s (Experiment 2), or extinction schedule (Experiment 3) during the other half. FI performance was well characterized by a mixture of two gamma-shaped distributions of responses. When a longer FI schedule was in effect in the first half of the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(84 reference statements)
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In contrast, the shape parameter of the gamma distribution was not significantly affected by the schedule or by a decrease in motivation. Furthermore, the notion that latencies are generated by a timing mechanism is consistent with previous research showing that latencies account for the curvature of FI cumulative records (Gentry, Weiss, & Laties, 1983), are roughly proportional to the duration of the FI (Lowe, Harzem, & Spencer, 1979;Lowe & Wearden, 1981;Shull, 1971;Wearden, 1985;Zeiler & Powell, 1994), and track rapid within-session changes in the duration of the FI (Higa, 1997;Ludvig & Staddon, 2004;Sanabria & Oldenburg, 2014;Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996).…”
Section: Latenciessupporting
confidence: 72%
“…In contrast, the shape parameter of the gamma distribution was not significantly affected by the schedule or by a decrease in motivation. Furthermore, the notion that latencies are generated by a timing mechanism is consistent with previous research showing that latencies account for the curvature of FI cumulative records (Gentry, Weiss, & Laties, 1983), are roughly proportional to the duration of the FI (Lowe, Harzem, & Spencer, 1979;Lowe & Wearden, 1981;Shull, 1971;Wearden, 1985;Zeiler & Powell, 1994), and track rapid within-session changes in the duration of the FI (Higa, 1997;Ludvig & Staddon, 2004;Sanabria & Oldenburg, 2014;Wynne, Staddon, & Delius, 1996).…”
Section: Latenciessupporting
confidence: 72%
“…Furthermore, readaptation to a short duration after a shift to longer duration is very rapid (Higa and Tillou 2001), which stands in contrast to our results, which could be interpreted as a fast learning of a longer duration and/or slow readaptation to a shorter duration. Previously reported asymmetries in adaptation to changing FI schedules may be partly explained through the learned anticipation of upcoming shifts, due to the repetition of shifts (Sanabria and Oldenburg 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Animals can learn time fast (Davis et al, 1989;Bevins and Ayres, 1995;Balsam et al, 2002;Diaz-Mataix et al, 2013), and adapt rapidly (i.e. within a few sessions) to changes in temporal rules in a peak interval protocol (Lejeune et al, 1997;Guilhardi et al, 2005;Sanabria et al, 2014;Dallérac et al, 2017). Similarly, animals can also adapt quickly to changes in a spatial rule from session to session (Morris et al, 1986;Blanco et al, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%