The use of a placebo in controlled trials is essential for the reliable assessment of a therapeutic drug. Sometimes trialists use an active placebo that can mimic the possible harms of the experimental drug with no therapeutic effects on the condition being treated. For example, active placebos, such as atropine, may be used in trials of antidepressants, where the drugs under investigation can cause noticeable harms and the risk of bias owing to unblinding may be high. 1 More commonly, placebos are intended to be inactive or inert. It should be impossible for participants, staff, and investigators to distinguish between the active drug and the placebo. To maintain blinding throughout the trial, inert placebos should match the sensory and visual aspects of the experimental drug and should be of equal shape, size, color, texture, weight, taste, and smell; a placebo is not simply a sugar pill. However, inactive placebos may contain excipients-chemicals, dyes, or allergens-that might unintentionally cause reactions or harms. 2 Inappropriately matching a placebo to the experimental drug can lead to underreporting of harms, confounding, or misleading trial outcomes.An example of this concern is a randomized trial reporting on the cardiovascular benefits of using a fishoil derivative (icosapent ethyl), in which the control group was given a placebo containing mineral oil to mimic the color and consistency of fish oil. 3 The participants taking the mineral-oil placebo had increases in some lipid and inflammation biomarkers, raising concerns that the cardiovascular benefits might have been an artifact of increased risk from the placebo, rather than a decreased risk from the fish-oil derivative. 4 Subsequently, a trial comparing omega-3 carboxylic acids with a placebo containing corn oil found no significant difference in cardiovascular events. 5 To clarify the uncertainty, a third trial with a placebo other than mineral oil would be needed.