2016
DOI: 10.1002/ece3.2489
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age‐dependent male mating tactics in a spider mite—A life‐history perspective

Abstract: Males often fight with rival males for access to females. However, some males display nonfighting tactics such as sneaking, satellite behavior, or female mimicking. When these mating tactics comprise a conditional strategy, they are often thought to be explained by resource holding potential (RHP), that is, nonfighting tactics are displayed by less competitive males who are more likely to lose a fight. The alternative mating tactics, however, can also be explained by life‐history theory, which predicts that yo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
16
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

5
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Fighting typically has lifetime consequences (Kemp, 2006; Sato et al ., 2016; Baxter and Dukas, 2017), and the life history of aggression with a dangerous fighting pattern was studied in A. disparis . Fighting in A. disparis is typically dangerous, resulting in severe injury or death (Liu et al ., 2017a; Liu and Hao, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Fighting typically has lifetime consequences (Kemp, 2006; Sato et al ., 2016; Baxter and Dukas, 2017), and the life history of aggression with a dangerous fighting pattern was studied in A. disparis . Fighting in A. disparis is typically dangerous, resulting in severe injury or death (Liu et al ., 2017a; Liu and Hao, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The signal of the ART of preceding guards may have come from cues left by the guards on the T‐female, or T‐females changed their smell in dependence of the guard, or some T‐females attracted more likely sneakers and others attracted more likely fighters. Dependency of adoption of a given ART on male age and density (Sato et al, ,) renders the explanation of ART‐specific cues left on T‐females more likely than the two other explanations. Males leaving chemical marks on their mates have been observed in various animals such as butterflies (Estrada et al, ) and fruit flies (Laturney & Billeter, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…ART expression by males is conditionally dichotomous and reversible, with fighting and sneaking phenotypes, and evident in male–male combat and pre‐copulatory guarding behaviour (Sato, Sabelis, Egas, & Faraji, ). Fighters and sneakers differ in behaviour but not in body size and morphology (Sato, Rühr, Schmitz, Egas, & Blanke, ; Sato et al, ). Mate finding occurs by males actively searching for females.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Fighter phenotypes fight with other males to gain access to, and monopolize, T-females and challenge guarding fighter males to obtain the guarding position. Sneaker phenotypes do not fight with other males and are cryptic to, and thus not fought by, other males [19,20,23]. Guarding is highly important for fertilization success since copulation takes place immediately upon the emergence of mature females.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%