1998
DOI: 10.2307/3677339
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Aggregate Dispersion of Birds after Encountering a Predator: Experimental Evidence

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
24
0
1

Year Published

2000
2000
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
3
24
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…This is consistent with the general presence of super-aggregation in adversity [1][2][3][4][5]7], but of course does not rule out additional mechanisms in particular situations, such as collective protection or confusion effects in the case of predation risk. Interestingly, our approach also makes predictions that are probably different from other explanations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is consistent with the general presence of super-aggregation in adversity [1][2][3][4][5]7], but of course does not rule out additional mechanisms in particular situations, such as collective protection or confusion effects in the case of predation risk. Interestingly, our approach also makes predictions that are probably different from other explanations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 64%
“…This phenomenon is well documented in groups of moving animals, which often form tighter groups in response to the detection of a predator [1][2][3][4][5][6]; but it also takes place in other adverse conditions, such as in the absence of food [7] or when animals are introduced into an unknown environment [8,9]. In humans, the occurrence of sudden bank runs [10][11][12] and human stampedes [13 -19] suggests increased aggregation in adversity, although data are insufficient to draw definitive conclusions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nevertheless, it is an interesting effect taking into account that the prey species studied are not considered to form inter-specific aggregations during the breeding season. Similar seasonal responses were found in forest passerines, which clumped their breeding territories when the perceived risk of predation increased (Forsman et al 1998). The tendency to form aggregations in our system may be accounted for by the lower probabilities of magpies attacking groups, and the lower chances of prey being caught if an attack occurred due to dilution effects (e.g., Whitfield 2003).…”
Section: Predation Patternssupporting
confidence: 59%
“…A stronger reaction to a more dangerous predator was observed in most of the studies (falcons vs. hawks vs. eagles vs. great horned owl Buteo virginianus, Buitron 1983; sparrowhawk vs. pygmy owl Glaucidium passerinum vs. tawny owl Strix aluco, Curio et al 1983; sparrowhawk vs. pygmy owl, Rytkönen and Soppela 1995;Forsman et al 1998). Griesser (2009) compared jay reactions to three hawk and three owl species (goshawk Accipiter gentilis, sparrowhawk Accipiter nisus, hobby Falco subbuteus, Ural owl Strix uralensi, hawk owl Surnia ulula, pygmy owl Glaucidium passerinum) of varying danger, and the jay's ability to communicate the threats.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 93%